Corpus callosum size and very preterm birth: relationship to neuropsychological outcome.

Thinning of the corpus callosum (CC) is often observed in individuals who were born very preterm. Damage to the CC during neurodevelopment may be associated with poor neuropsychological performance. This study aimed to explore any evidence of CC pathology in adolescents aged 14-15 years who were born very preterm, and to investigate the relationship between CC areas and verbal skills. Seventy-two individuals born before 33 weeks of gestation and 51 age- and sex-matched full-term controls received structural MRI and neuropsychological assessment. Total CC area in very preterm adolescents was 7.5% smaller than in controls, after adjusting for total white matter volume (P = 0.015). The absolute size of callosal subregions differed between preterm and full-term adolescents: preterm individuals had a 14.7% decrease in posterior (P < 0.0001) and an 11.6% decrease in mid-posterior CC quarters (P = 0.029). Preterm individuals who had experienced periventricular haemorrhage and ventricular dilatation in the neonatal period showed the greatest decrease in CC area. In very preterm boys only, verbal IQ and verbal fluency scores were positively associated with total mid-sagittal CC size and mid-posterior surface area. These results suggest that very preterm birth adversely affects the development of the CC, particularly its posterior quarter, and this impairs verbal skills in boys.

[1]  R. Holloway,,et al.  Sexual dimorphism in the human corpus callosum. , 1982, Science.

[2]  R. Defendini,et al.  Sexual dimorphism of the human corpus callosum from three independent samples: relative size of the corpus callosum. , 1993, American journal of physical anthropology.

[3]  F. J. Schonell,et al.  Diagnostic and attainment testing : including a manual of tests, their nature, use, recording and interpretation , 1950 .

[4]  K. Flannery,et al.  Male prevalence for reading disability is found in a large sample of Black and White children free from ascertainment bias , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[5]  Judith M. Rumsey,et al.  Corpus callosum morphology, as measured with MRI, in dyslexic men , 1996, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  E. Ross,et al.  Topography of the Human Corpus Callosum , 1985, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[7]  J A Wada,et al.  Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in humans. Cortical speech zones in 100 adults and 100 infant brains. , 1975, Archives of neurology.

[8]  M. Bryden,et al.  Gender, laterality, learning difficulties and health problems , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  D. Miller,et al.  Brain structure and neurocognitive and behavioural function in adolescents who were born very preterm , 1999, The Lancet.

[10]  D. Ffytche,et al.  The functional significance of perinatal corpus callosum damage: an fMRI study in young adults. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  M Lassonde,et al.  Callosal and cortical contribution to procedural learning. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  W. Cumming An anatomical review of the corpus callosum. , 1970, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[13]  S. F. Witelson The brain connection: the corpus callosum is larger in left-handers. , 1985, Science.

[14]  A. Edwards,et al.  RELATION BETWEEN ULTRASOUND APPEARANCE OF THE BRAIN OF VERY PRETERM INFANTS AND NEURODEVELOPMENTAL IMPARIMENT AT EIGHT YEARS , 1993, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[15]  Jagath C. Rajapakse,et al.  Development of the human corpus callosum during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[16]  Chiara Nosarti,et al.  Adolescents who were born very preterm have decreased brain volumes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  P W Woodruff,et al.  A computerized magnetic resonance imaging study of corpus callosum morphology in schizophrenia , 1993, Psychological Medicine.

[18]  D. Mccormick,et al.  107 MOTOR TESTS OF INTERHEMISPHERIC CONTROL AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION IN VERY PRETERM INFANTS AT EIGHT YEARS , 1994, Pediatric Research.

[19]  M. Langmeier,et al.  Changes of the neuronal structure produced by prolonged hypobaric hypoxia in infant rats. , 1989, Biomedica biochimica acta.

[20]  Christopher J. Cannistraci,et al.  Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. , 2000, JAMA.

[21]  Timothy Schallert,et al.  Plasticity‐Associated Molecular and Structural Events in the Injured Brain , 2002, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[22]  F. Fabbro,et al.  A callosal transfer deficit in children with developmental language disorder , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  A. Elster,et al.  Sexual dimorphism of the human corpus callosum studied by magnetic resonance imaging: Fact, fallacy and statistical confidence , 1990, Brain and Development.

[24]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Cognitive and sensori-motor functioning in the absence of the corpus callosum: Neuropsychological studies in callosal agenesis and callosotomized patients , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  N. Minshew,et al.  Development of the corpus callosum in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood. , 2002, Life sciences.

[26]  E. Wenger,et al.  The controversy about a sexual dimorphism of the human corpus callosum. , 1989, The International journal of neuroscience.

[27]  O. Selnes A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests , 1991, Neurology.

[28]  P. Crichton,et al.  Verbal fluency in schizophrenia: relationship with executive function, semantic memory and clinical alogia , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  S. F. Witelson Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of the human corpus callosum. A postmortem morphological study. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  G. Herman,et al.  3D Imaging In Medicine , 1991 .

[31]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Lateralisation of language function in young adults born very preterm , 2004, Archives of Disease in Childhood - Fetal and Neonatal Edition.

[32]  H. Kinney,et al.  Late Oligodendrocyte Progenitors Coincide with the Developmental Window of Vulnerability for Human Perinatal White Matter Injury , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Are men's and women's brains really different? , 1987 .

[34]  S. Pizer,et al.  3D Imaging in Medicine , 1990, NATO ASI Series.

[35]  A. Toga,et al.  Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive correlates , 2001, Neurology.

[36]  R W Cooke,et al.  Cranial magnetic resonance imaging and school performance in very low birth weight infants in adolescence , 1999, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[37]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Preterm Children Have Disturbances of White Matter at 11 Years of Age as Shown by Diffusion Tensor Imaging , 2003, Pediatric Research.

[38]  E. Reynolds,et al.  PREDICTION OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL IMPAIRMENT AT FOUR YEARS FROM BRAIN ULTRASOUND APPEARANCE OF VERY PRETERM INFANTS , 1988, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[39]  J. Wyatt,et al.  Neuropsychological outcome at adolescence of very preterm birth and its relation to brain structure. , 2001, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[40]  J. Wyatt,et al.  PROBABILITÉ OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS ESTIMATED FROM ULTRASOUND APPEARANCE OF BRAINS OF VERY PRETERM INFANTS , 1987, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[41]  H. Lubs,et al.  Neuroanatomic differences between dyslexic and normal readers on magnetic resonance imaging scans. , 1991, Archives of neurology.