Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics?
暂无分享,去创建一个
Christophe Bourdin | Jean-Louis Vercher | Lionel Bringoux | Nicole Malfait | J. Vercher | F. Sarlegna | C. Bourdin | N. Malfait | L. Bringoux | Fabrice R. Sarlegna
[1] J. Lackner,et al. Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis force perturbations of their reaching movements. , 2000, Journal of Neurophysiology.
[2] Jinsung Wang,et al. Adaptation to Visuomotor Rotations Remaps Movement Vectors, Not Final Positions , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] R. Miall,et al. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] C. Bard,et al. Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] M. Latash,et al. Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[6] N. Teasdale,et al. Vestibular signal processing in a subject with somatosensory deafferentation: The case of sitting posture , 2007, BMC neurology.
[7] J. Lackner,et al. Motor control and learning in altered dynamic environments , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[8] Christopher A. Buneo,et al. On the form of the internal model for reaching , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] David J Ostry,et al. Modifiability of generalization in dynamics learning. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] J. Vercher,et al. Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.
[11] C. Sherrington,et al. VIII. Experiments upon the influence of sensory nerves upon movement and nutrition of the limbs. Preliminary communication , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.
[12] Lamarre,et al. Self-moved target eye tracking in control and deafferented subjects: roles of arm motor command and proprioception in arm-eye coordination. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[13] Y. Lamarre,et al. Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[14] R. Sainburg,et al. The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[15] Hao Sun,et al. The temporal properties of the response of macaque ganglion cells and central mechanisms of flicker detection. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[16] Howard Poizner,et al. Influence of movement speed on accuracy and coordination of reaching movements to memorized targets in three-dimensional space in a deafferented subject , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] L. Merabet,et al. The plastic human brain cortex. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[18] R. Shadmehr,et al. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.
[19] Proprioceptive deafferentation slows down the processing of visual hand feedback. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[20] J. Vercher,et al. Vision of the hand prior to movement onset allows full motor adaptation to a multi-force environment , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.
[21] J. Sanes,et al. Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. , 1985, Human neurobiology.
[22] J C Rothwell,et al. Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[23] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[24] Fabrice R. Sarlegna,et al. Impairment of online control of reaching movements with aging: A double-step study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[25] Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] C. Bourdin,et al. Visual feedback of the moving arm allows complete adaptation of pointing movements to centrifugal and Coriolis forces in human subjects , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.
[27] J. Lackner,et al. Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] Gary W Thickbroom,et al. Dual representation of the hand in the cerebellum: activation with voluntary and passive finger movement , 2003, NeuroImage.
[29] E. Sedgwick,et al. The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.
[30] G. Gauthier,et al. Oculo-manual tracking of visual targets in monkey: role of the arm afferent information in the control of arm and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] E. Naito,et al. Visuokinesthetic Perception of Hand Movement is Mediated by Cerebro–Cerebellar Interaction between the Left Cerebellum and Right Parietal Cortex , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[33] C. Prablanc,et al. Pointing movement in an artificial perturbing inertial field: A prospective paradigm for motor control study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[34] Jean-Louis Vercher,et al. The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] J. D. Cooke,et al. Initial agonist burst duration changes with movement amplitude in a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Lack of conscious recognition of one's own actions in a haptically deafferented patient , 2002, Neuroreport.
[38] Matthew W. G. Dye,et al. Do deaf individuals see better? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[39] R L Sainburg,et al. Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] C. Bard,et al. Control of single-joint movements in deafferented patients: evidence for amplitude coding rather than position control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] C. Bard,et al. Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[42] Fabrice R Sarlegna,et al. Influence of Feedback Modality on Sensorimotor Adaptation: Contribution of Visual, Kinesthetic, and Verbal Cues , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.
[43] V. Gurfinkel,et al. Proprioceptive consequences of tendon vibration during movement. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[44] John W. Krakauer,et al. Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[45] E. Bizzi,et al. Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.
[46] E Bizzi,et al. Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[47] Dagmar Sternad,et al. Progress in Motor Control :A Multidisciplinary Perspective , 2009 .
[48] M. Kawato,et al. Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics , 2007, PloS one.
[49] E. Taub,et al. Use of Sensory Recombination and Somatosensory Deafferentation Techniques in the Investigation of Sensory-Motor Integration , 1974, Perception.
[50] T. Ebner,et al. Hereditary cerebellar ataxia progressively impairs force adaptation during goal-directed arm movements. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[51] D. Wolpert,et al. Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[52] C. Ghez,et al. Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[53] Ian M Franks,et al. Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.
[54] B. Day,et al. Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the absence of somatosensory information. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[55] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Kinematics and Dynamics Are Not Represented Independently in Motor Working Memory: Evidence from an Interference Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[56] M. Hallett,et al. PET study of visually and non-visually guided finger movements in patients with severe pan-sensory neuropathies and healthy controls , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[57] Andrew A G Mattar,et al. Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.
[58] F. W. Mott,et al. Experiments on the Influence of Sensory Nerves upon Movement and Nutrition of the Limbs. , 1895 .
[59] M. Mishkin,et al. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. , 1991, Science.
[60] M. Harvey,et al. Age-related differences in corrected and inhibited pointing movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[61] A. McIntosh,et al. Time course of changes in brain activity and functional connectivity associated with long-term adaptation to a rotational transformation. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] C. Bard,et al. Weight judgment. The discrimination capacity of a deafferented subject. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[63] R. Shadmehr,et al. Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington's disease but not cerebellar degeneration. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[64] J. Krakauer,et al. Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[65] Reza Shadmehr,et al. The Computational Neurobiology of Reaching and Pointing: A Foundation for Motor Learning , 2004 .
[66] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Motor Adaptation Scaled by the Difficulty of a Secondary Cognitive Task , 2008, PloS one.
[67] J. Gordon,et al. Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.