COPY AND RECALL PERFORMANCE OF 6–8-YEAR-OLD CHILDREN AFTER STANDARD VS. STEP-BY-STEP ADMINISTRATION OF THE REY-OSTERRIETH COMPLEX FIGURE

The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF; Rey 1941; Osterrieth, 1944) is frequently used in the neuropsychological assessment of children and adults. The present study was designed, in part, to examine the impact of providing organizational scaffolding to young children being tested with the ROCF. To this end, 6-, 7-, and 8-year–old children were administered the test either in the standard fashion, or using a format in which the 18 key elements of the figure were introduced sequentially. Participants included 132 children who were randomly assigned to the standard or step-by-step administration groups. Significantly higher accuracy and organization scores for both copy and recall were seen with the step-by-step format than with the standard format, even though children in the step-by-step condition took less time to execute their drawings. Retention of encoded information was not affected by age or testing format. The fact that 6-year-olds in the step-by-step condition performed as well as, or better than, 8-year-olds in the standard condition suggests that the primary problem young children experience with the ROCF lies with organizational strategy formation. Advantages of using the Step-by-step ROCF in clinical practice are discussed. This research was supported by NSERC grant no. 138829-01 to LSJ. The authors would like to thank the children who participated in this research, and their families, for their time and effort.

[1]  Kris Fernando,et al.  Standardisation of the Rey Complex Figure Test in New Zealand Children and Adolescents , 2003 .

[2]  J. Stiles,et al.  The impact of early unilateral brain injury on perceptual organization and visual memory , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  D. Tremblay,et al.  Comparability of Total Score Performance on the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure and a Modified Taylor Complex Figure , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[4]  L S Jakobson,et al.  The relationship between periventricular brain injury and deficits in visual processing among extremely-low-birthweight (< 1000 g) children. , 2001, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[5]  A. Toga,et al.  Mapping Continued Brain Growth and Gray Matter Density Reduction in Dorsal Frontal Cortex: Inverse Relationships during Postadolescent Brain Maturation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  M. D. Weiler,et al.  Sources of Poor Performance on the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test among Children With Learning Difficulties: A Dynamic Assessment Approach , 2001, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[7]  D. Delis,et al.  Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: A structural MRI study , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[8]  W. Burns,et al.  Assessment of Visual-Motor Coordination in 6-to 11-yr.-Olds , 2000, Perceptual and motor skills.

[9]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Henderson,et al.  The Effect of Strategy on the Recall of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure in Children with or without Learning Disabilities , 1999 .

[12]  A. Toga,et al.  Localizing Age-Related Changes in Brain Structure between Childhood and Adolescence Using Statistical Parametric Mapping , 1999, NeuroImage.

[13]  S. Rauch,et al.  Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  S. Warschausky,et al.  WISC-III index growth curve characteristics following traumatic brain injury. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children--Third Edition. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[15]  N. Denburg,et al.  Adult norms for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and for supplemental recognition and matching trials from the Extended Complex Figure Test. , 1999, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[16]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  Further MRI evidence of late brain maturation : Limbic volume increases and changing asymmetries during childhood and adolescence , 1998 .

[17]  F. Vlachos,et al.  Sex and Handedness in Development of Visuomotor Skills , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.

[18]  H. Whyte,et al.  PREDICTING LONG-TERM OUTCOME IN SMALL FOR GESTATIONAL AGE (SGA) CHILDREN† 1163 , 1997, Pediatric Research.

[19]  N. Denburg,et al.  Intentional and incidental memory: Order effects in clinical testing. , 1997 .

[20]  P. Fastenau,et al.  Development and preliminary standardization of the "extended complex figure test" (ECFT). , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[21]  J. Stiles,et al.  Developmental Trends in Visuospatial Analysis and Planning: I. Copying a Complex Figure , 1995 .

[22]  J. Stiles,et al.  Developmental Trends in Visuospatial Analysis and Planning: II. Memory for a Complex Figure , 1995 .

[23]  J. Meyers,et al.  Rey complex figure test under four different administration procedures , 1995 .

[24]  J. Meyers,et al.  Recognition Subtest for the Complex Figure , 1994 .

[25]  F. Benes,et al.  Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[26]  D. Tzuriel,et al.  Structured program of visual-motor integration (SP-VMI) for preschool children , 1992 .

[27]  T. Jernigan,et al.  Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  A. Karapetsas,et al.  Visuomotor Organization in the Child: A Neuropsychological Approach , 1991, Perceptual and motor skills.

[29]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[30]  J. Holmes,et al.  Assessing children's copy productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. , 1985, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[31]  B. Pillon Troubles visuo-constructifs et méthodes de compensation: Résultats de 85 patients atteints de lésions cérébrales , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Yaacov Rand,et al.  The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The learning potential assessment device, theory, instruments and techniques , 1981 .

[33]  F. Lhermitte,et al.  [Neuropsychological analysis of the frontal syndrome]. , 1972, Revue neurologique.

[34]  P. Yakovlev,et al.  The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .

[35]  Jane Holmes Bernstein,et al.  Performance of learning‐disabled and non‐learning‐disabled children on the Rey‐Osterrieth Complex Figure: Validation of the developmental scoring system , 1995 .

[36]  T. Moffitt,et al.  The Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test: Norms for young adolescents and an examination of validity , 1995 .

[37]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[38]  P. Isquith,et al.  Metacognitive factors in the visuospatial skills of long‐term survivors of acute lymphoblastic leukemia: An experimental approach to the Rey‐Osterrieth complex figure test , 1994 .

[39]  A. Rey,et al.  Translations of excerpts from Andre Rey"s Psychological examination of traumatic encephalopathy and P. A. Osterrieth"s The Complex Figure Copy Test.. , 1993 .

[40]  June Corwin,et al.  Psychological examination of traumatic encephalopathy , 1993 .

[41]  D. Berry,et al.  Effect of four different delay periods on recall of the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure by older persons , 1992 .

[42]  D. Delis,et al.  Memory for the Rey-Osterrieth Figure: Perceptual clustering, encoding, and storage. , 1992 .