Reading and solving arithmetic problems improves cognitive functions of normal aged people: a randomized controlled study

The relationship between mental exercise and mental aging is a controversial issue. People generally believe the so-called mental-exercise hypothesis, that is, the age-related decline in cognitive function is less pronounced for people who are mentally active, yet there is insufficient scientific evidence supporting this hypothesis. Previous randomized controlled trial studies showed convincing beneficial effects of cognitive training on directly targeted cognitive functions. In this study, we performed a single-blind, randomized controlled trial on cognitive intervention in 124 community-dwelling seniors (age range, 70 to 86) and estimated the beneficial effects of non-targeted cognitive functions. As for cognitive intervention, the subjects were asked to solve systematized basic problems in reading and arithmetic every day for 6 months. Neuropsychological measures were determined prior to and 6 months after the intervention (post-test) by mini-mental state examination (MMSE), frontal assessment battery at bed side (FAB), and digit-symbol substitution test (DST) of WAIS-R. The FAB and DST scores showed a statistically significant (p<0.001 and p<0.01, respectively) improvement in the post-test compared with the pre-test, such improvement was maintained up to 6 months of follow-up tests in only the experimental group. The transfer effect of cognitive intervention by reading and solving arithmetic problems on non-targeted cognitive functions was demonstrated in this study. This study shows that daily mental training can improve cognitive functions in normal adults. Although general interests in brain training have been increasing in the public, evidence for its beneficial effects, particularly the positive transfer effect on non-targeted cognitive function still remains insufficient. Here, we introduce a new cognitive intervention program for normal aged people, the concept of which is derived from the knowledge of both brain science and clinical studies. We performed a carefully designed single-blind, randomized controlled study, and the results of this study showed convincing evidence that cognitive training provides the beneficial transfer effect.

[1]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. , 1955 .

[2]  Atsushi Hozawa,et al.  Association between social support and depression status in the elderly: Results of a 1‐year community‐based prospective cohort study in Japan , 2005, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[3]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  Male elderly subthreshold depression patients have smaller volume of medial part of prefrontal cortex and precentral gyrus compared with age-matched normal subjects: a voxel-based morphometry. , 2005, Journal of affective disorders.

[4]  P E Roland,et al.  Localization of cortical areas activated by thinking. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[6]  Naoki Miura,et al.  Cortical activation during reading aloud of long sentences: fMRI study , 2003, Neuroreport.

[7]  Masato Taira,et al.  Reading aloud and arithmetic calculation improve frontal function of people with dementia. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[8]  J. Grafman,et al.  Visualizing Cortical Activation during Mental Calculation with Functional MRI , 1996, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Grafman,et al.  The calculating brain: an fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  M. Mintun,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during word and nonword reading , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[11]  I Litvan,et al.  The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside , 2000, Neurology.

[12]  R Zemke,et al.  Occupational therapy for independent-living older adults. A randomized controlled trial. , 1997, JAMA.

[13]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic--a comparison between children and adults. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[14]  B Bioulac,et al.  Lateralization of prefrontal activation during internal mental calculation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  Mental Exercise and Mental Aging the Validity of the ''Use It or Lose It'' Hypothesis , 2006 .

[16]  D. Kreiner,et al.  Memory and Motor Skill Components of the WAIS-III Digit Symbol-Coding Subtest , 2001, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[17]  Deborah Fein,et al.  Decoding Digit Symbol , 2003, Assessment.

[18]  Christina Wasylyshyn,et al.  Adult age and digit symbol substitution performance: a meta-analysis. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[19]  A. Wingfield,et al.  Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  P. Nichelli,et al.  The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): normative values in an Italian population sample , 2005, Neurological Sciences.

[21]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociating Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex Activation during Arithmetic Processing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[22]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: a PET study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  George W Rebok,et al.  Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. , 2002, JAMA.

[24]  Sharona M. Atkins,et al.  Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: A randomized, controlled study , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  J. Touchon,et al.  Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment , 2001, Neurology.

[26]  M. Lezak,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment, 4th ed. , 2004 .

[27]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Achieving and maintaining cognitive vitality with aging. , 2002, Mayo Clinic proceedings.

[28]  W. Cunningham,et al.  Age-related slowing on the Digit Symbol task: longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses. , 1999, Experimental aging research.

[29]  J. Millichap,et al.  Brain Activity During Reading , 1995 .

[30]  K. Schaie,et al.  Training the elderly on the ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning. , 1986, Psychology and aging.

[31]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Brain activity during reading. The effects of exposure duration and task. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Makoto Takahashi,et al.  Cortical activation during reading of ancient versus modern Japanese texts: fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[33]  H. Arai,et al.  Increased plasma 8-isoprostane levels in hypertensive subjects: the Tsurugaya Project. , 2004, Hypertension research : official journal of the Japanese Society of Hypertension.

[34]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  The Neural Circuitry Involved in the Reading of German Words and Pseudowords: A PET Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  K. Ball,et al.  Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. , 2006, JAMA.