The role of awareness in shaping responses in human visual cortex
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Deouell,et al. Neuroscientific Evidence for Processing Without Awareness. , 2022, Annual review of neuroscience.
[2] M. Sereno,et al. Topological Maps and Brain Computations From Low to High , 2022, Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience.
[3] D. S. Schwarzkopf,et al. The human primary visual cortex (V1) encodes the perceived position of static but not moving objects , 2021, Communications Biology.
[4] P. Winklewski,et al. Critical Flicker Fusion Frequency: A Narrative Review , 2021, Medicina.
[5] Roberto Santana,et al. Informative neural representations of unseen contents during higher-order processing in human brains and deep artificial networks , 2021, Nature Human Behaviour.
[6] Nonie Finlayson,et al. Topographic signatures of global object perception in human visual cortex , 2019, NeuroImage.
[7] J. Dubois,et al. Imaging object-scene relations processing in visible and invisible natural scenes , 2019, Scientific Reports.
[8] Ying Wang,et al. Subconscious processing reveals dissociable contextual modulations of visual size perception , 2018, Cognition.
[9] Rainer Goebel,et al. Reconstructing imagined letters from early visual cortex reveals tight topographic correspondence between visual mental imagery and perception , 2018, Brain Structure and Function.
[10] Y. Sugita,et al. Size-contrast illusion induced by unconscious context. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[11] L. Mudrik,et al. Evidence for Implicit—But Not Unconscious—Processing of Object-Scene Relations , 2018, Psychological science.
[12] P. R. Montoro,et al. Global shape integration and illusory form perception in the absence of awareness , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[13] Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort,et al. Perceptual integration without conscious access , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[14] Johan Wagemans,et al. Scene Integration Without Awareness , 2016, Psychological science.
[15] D. Schwarzkopf,et al. Induction of Kanizsa Contours Requires Awareness of the Inducing Context , 2016, bioRxiv.
[16] J. Wagemans,et al. No evidence for surface organization in Kanizsa configurations during continuous flash suppression , 2015, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[17] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Cortical activity is more stable when sensory stimuli are consciously perceived , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] F. D. Lange,et al. Shape Perception Simultaneously Up- and Downregulates Neural Activity in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2014, Current Biology.
[19] Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort,et al. Seeing without Knowing: Neural Signatures of Perceptual Inference in the Absence of Report , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] C. Koch,et al. Differential processing of invisible congruent and incongruent scenes: a case for unconscious integration. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[21] Victor A. F. Lamme,et al. Confuse Your Illusion , 2013, Psychological science.
[22] Xuchu Weng,et al. Perceptual Grouping without Awareness: Superiority of Kanizsa Triangle in Breaking Interocular Suppression , 2012, PloS one.
[23] Jonas Larsson,et al. fMRI repetition suppression: neuronal adaptation or stimulus expectation? , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[24] P. Sterzer,et al. Breaking Continuous Flash Suppression: A New Measure of Unconscious Processing during Interocular Suppression? , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[25] Dominique Lamy,et al. Integration without awareness: expanding the limits of unconscious processing , 2011 .
[26] Dominique Lamy,et al. Integration Without Awareness , 2011, Psychological science.
[27] D. Schwarzkopf,et al. Contextual Illusions Reveal the Limit of Unconscious Visual Processing , 2011, Psychological science.
[28] D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al. Interpreting local visual features as a global shape requires awareness , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[29] Huafu Chen,et al. Neural mechanism of unconscious perception of surprised facial expression , 2010, NeuroImage.
[30] D. Macleod,et al. Imperceptibly rapid contrast modulations processed in cortex: Evidence from psychophysics. , 2010, Journal of Vision.
[31] B. Wandell,et al. Mapping Hv4 and Ventral Occipital Cortex: the Venous Eclipse , 2022 .
[32] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Reproducibility Distinguishes Conscious from Nonconscious Neural Representations , 2010, Science.
[33] R. Blake,et al. Suppression During Binocular Rivalry Broadens Orientation Tuning , 2009, Psychological science.
[34] G. Rees,et al. Fine-scale activity patterns in high-level visual areas encode the category of invisible objects. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[35] Brian A. Wandell,et al. Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex , 2008, NeuroImage.
[36] Edward Vul,et al. Contingent aftereffects distinguish conscious and preconscious color processing , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[37] C. Koch,et al. Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] G. Rees,et al. Predicting the orientation of invisible stimuli from activity in human primary visual cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[39] Justin A. Harris,et al. Contextual Modulation outside of Awareness , 2005, Current Biology.
[40] Patrik Vuilleumier,et al. Effects of Low-Spatial Frequency Components of Fearful Faces on Fusiform Cortex Activity , 2003, Current Biology.
[41] N. Logothetis. The Underpinnings of the BOLD Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Signal , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[42] S Zeki,et al. The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[43] J B Poline,et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[44] Sheng He,et al. Orientation-selective adaptation and tilt after-effect from invisible patterns , 2001, Nature.
[45] R. Dolan,et al. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[46] Anders M. Dale,et al. Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.
[47] M. Livingstone,et al. Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[48] S. Rauch,et al. Masked Presentations of Emotional Facial Expressions Modulate Amygdala Activity without Explicit Knowledge , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[49] W. Newsome,et al. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[50] Edward H. Adelson,et al. The delayed rod afterimage , 1982, Vision Research.
[51] C. Spearman. CORRELATION CALCULATED FROM FAULTY DATA , 1910 .
[52] C. Koch,et al. Low-level awareness accompanies "unconscious" high-level processing during continuous flash suppression. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[53] Omar H. Butt,et al. The retinotopic organization of striate cortex is well predicted by surface topology Supplemental Information , 2012 .