DECISION-MAKING IN GROUP FORAGERS WITH INCOMPLETE INFORMATION: TEST OF INDIVIDUAL-BASED MODEL IN GEESE

One important challenge of spatial ecology is to generate models linking individual behavior to population-level phenomena. Although animals often face great uncertainty regarding foraging patch quality, earlier models explaining the aggregation of animals have rarely specified how stable outcomes are achieved through individual decisions, especially under realistic assumptions for incompletely informed foragers. We developed a new foraging model that assumed a realistic decision-making rule for incompletely informed group foragers, and we tested its performance against existing models with different assumptions by comparing how well they reproduce the patterns observed in foraging White-fronted Geese (Anser albifrons). The assumptions in each of the four compared models were: (1) incompletely informed foraging with benefits of group foraging, which uses the expected gain rates for making decisions on diet choice, patch departure, and flock joining; (2) incompletely informed foraging without benefits of g...

[1]  M. Owen,et al.  Energy budgets of wintering Barnacle Geese: the effects of declining food resources , 1992 .

[2]  H. Higuchi,et al.  Foraging Patch Selection and Departure by Non‐Omniscient Foragers: A Field Example in White‐Fronted Geese , 2006 .

[3]  Steven F. Railsback,et al.  ANALYSIS OF HABITAT‐SELECTION RULES USING ANINDIVIDUAL‐BASED MODEL , 2002 .

[4]  L. Edelstein-Keshet,et al.  Complexity, pattern, and evolutionary trade-offs in animal aggregation. , 1999, Science.

[5]  Michael J. Conroy,et al.  Parameter Estimation, Reliability, and Model Improvement for Spatially Explicit Models of Animal Populations , 1995 .

[6]  R. May,et al.  Aggregation of Predators and Insect Parasites and its Effect on Stability , 1974 .

[7]  R. R. Krausz Living in Groups , 2013 .

[8]  M. Mangel,et al.  An ecosystem-based approach to management: using individual behaviour to predict the indirect effects of Antarctic krill fisheries on penguin foraging , 2003 .

[9]  Richard A. Stillman,et al.  Carrying capacity in overwintering migratory birds , 2002 .

[10]  Wolf M. Mooij,et al.  Search paths of swans foraging on spatially autocorrelated tubers , 2002 .

[11]  SIZE-RELATED EFFECTS ON SEARCH TIMES IN NORTH AMERICAN GRASSLAND FEMALE UNGULATES' , 1988 .

[12]  N. J. Frost,et al.  Predicting site quality for shorebird communities:a case study on the Humber estuary, UK , 2005 .

[13]  Andrew D. West,et al.  Predicting mortality in novel environments: tests and sensitivity of a behaviour‐based model , 2000 .

[14]  Volker Grimm,et al.  Individual-based modelling and ecological theory: synthesis of a workshop , 1999 .

[15]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Towards a behavioral ecology of ecological landscapes. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[16]  W. Sutherland From Individual Behaviour to Population Ecology , 1996 .

[17]  C. Carbone,et al.  Foraging inequalities in large groups: quantifying depletion experienced by individuals in goose flocks , 2004 .

[18]  G. Gauthier,et al.  Comparison of daily energy expenditure of greater snow geese between two habitats , 1984 .

[19]  H. Higuchi,et al.  Factors affecting rice grain density unconsumed by white-fronted geese in relation to wheat damage , 2004 .

[20]  Costs and benefits of flocking in foraging white‐fronted geese (Anser albifrons): effects of resource depletion , 2006 .

[21]  M. Owen Abdominal Profile: A Condition Index for Wild Geese in the Field , 1981 .

[22]  Ian M. Hamilton,et al.  GROUP FORAGING BY A KLEPTOPARASITIC FISH: A STRONG INFERENCE TEST OF SOCIAL FORAGING MODELS , 2003 .

[23]  D. L. Orthmeyer,et al.  A Pacific spring migration route and breeding range expansion for greater white-fronted geese wintering in Japan , 2000 .

[24]  C. Bernstein,et al.  Information acquisition and time allocation in insect parasitoids , 2003 .

[25]  William J. Sutherland,et al.  A spatial depletion model of the interaction between bean geese and wigeon with the consequences for habitat management , 1994 .

[26]  Clive W. Anderson,et al.  Predicting the Distribution of Individuals and the Consequences of Habitat Loss: The Role of Prey Depletion , 1993 .

[27]  B. Pinshow,et al.  Density-dependent habitat selection in migratory passerines during stopover: what causes the deviation from IFD? , 2002, Evolutionary Ecology.

[28]  J. Krebs,et al.  INDIVIDUAL DECISIONS AND THE DISTRIBUTION OF PREDATORS IN A PATCHY ENVIRONMENT , 1988 .

[29]  J. Alonso,et al.  Patch use in cranes: a field test of optimal foraging predictions , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  T. Caraco,et al.  Social Foraging Theory , 2018 .

[31]  M. Bélisle FORAGING GROUP SIZE: MODELS AND A TEST WITH JAEGERS KLEPTOPARASITIZING TERNS , 1998 .

[32]  H. Higuchi,et al.  Alleviating grazing damage by white‐fronted geese: an optimal foraging approach , 2004 .

[33]  J. M. Black,et al.  Food intake, body reserves and reproductive success of barnacle geese Branta leucopsis staging in different habitats , 1998 .

[34]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus , 1990 .

[35]  A. D. Fox,et al.  Spring migration routes and timing of Greenland white-fronted geese – results from satellite telemetry , 2003 .

[36]  A. Houston,et al.  The Common Currency for Behavioral Decisions , 1986, The American Naturalist.

[37]  B. Danielson,et al.  Spatially Explicit Population Models: Current Forms and Future Uses , 1995 .

[38]  William W. Hargrove,et al.  Predicting spatial distribution of foragers over large resource landscapes : a modeling analysis of the Ideal Free Distribution , 1997 .

[39]  F. Arengo,et al.  Effects of food density on the behavior and distribution of nonbreeding American Flamingos in Yucatan, Mexico , 1995 .

[40]  C. Perrins,et al.  Wild Geese of the World , 1980 .

[41]  G. Parker,et al.  Competition for resources , 1991 .

[42]  J. Godin,et al.  Context-dependent group size choice in fish , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  E. Charnov Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.

[44]  M. Abrahams,et al.  Patch choice under perceptual constraints: a cause for departures from an ideal free distribution , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[45]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  Spatially explicit, individual-based, behavioural models of the annual cycle of two migratory goose populations , 2000 .

[46]  L. Giraldeau Chapter 3 – THE STABLE GROUP AND THE DETERMINANTS OF FORAGING GROUP SIZE1 , 1988 .

[47]  G. A. Parker,et al.  Ideal free distributions when individuals differ in competitive ability: phenotype-limited ideal free models , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[48]  R. Pettifor,et al.  Modelling the dynamics of winter barnacle goose flocks : A progress report , 1998 .

[49]  J. Lazarus Vigilance, flock size and domain of danger size in the White-fronted Goose , 1978 .

[50]  A. Persson,et al.  The effects of spatial food distribution and group size on foraging behaviour in a benthic fish , 2005, Behavioural Processes.

[51]  John M. Fryxell,et al.  Forage Quality and Aggregation by Large Herbivores , 1991, The American Naturalist.

[52]  G. Gauthier,et al.  ARE ABDOMINAL PROFILES USEFUL TO ASSESS BODY CONDITION OF SPRING STAGING GREATER SNOW GEESE? , 2005 .

[53]  E. Marschall,et al.  A dynamic model of group-size choice in the coral reef fish Dascyllus albisella , 1999 .

[54]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  The group-size paradox: effects of learning and patch departure rules , 2005 .

[55]  Michael M. Delm Vigilance for predators: detection and dilution effects , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[56]  A. Houston,et al.  State-dependent ideal free distributions , 1990, Evolutionary Ecology.

[57]  Z. Barta,et al.  The effect of energy reserves on social foraging: hungry sparrows scrounge more , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  B. Ebbinge,et al.  The importance of body reserves accumulated in spring staging areas in the temperate zone for breeding in Dark-bellied Brent Geese Branta b. bernicla in the high Arctic , 1995 .

[59]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Learning rules for social foragers: implications for the producer-scrounger game and ideal free distribution theory. , 2000, Journal of theoretical biology.

[60]  Colin A. Chapman,et al.  Producers, Scroungers, and Group Foraging , 1991, The American Naturalist.

[61]  R. Sibly,et al.  Producers and scroungers: A general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[62]  Volker Grimm,et al.  Using pattern-oriented modeling for revealing hidden information: a key for reconciling ecological theory and application , 2003 .

[63]  Steven F. Railsback,et al.  Individual-based modeling and ecology , 2005 .

[64]  T. Caraco,et al.  Ecological Determinants of Group Sizes of Foraging Lions , 1975, The American Naturalist.

[65]  D. Macdonald,et al.  Small pack size imposes a trade-off between hunting and pup-guarding in the painted hunting dog Lycaon pictus , 2002 .

[66]  Theunis Piersma,et al.  Incompletely Informed Shorebirds That Face a Digestive Constraint Maximize Net Energy Gain When Exploiting Patches , 2003, The American Naturalist.

[67]  William J. Sutherland,et al.  A Spatial Depletion Model of the Responses of Grazing Wildfowl to the Availability of Intertidal Vegetation , 1996 .

[68]  R. Walters,et al.  The impact of disturbance on the behaviour and energy budgets of Brent Geese Branta b. bernicla , 1996 .

[69]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Influence of Conspecific Attraction on the Spatial Distribution of Learning Foragers in a Patchy Habitat. , 1997 .