Does a salient distractor capture attention early in processing?

Kim and Cave (1999) used spatial probes in order to measure the effects of bottom-up and top-down factors on the allocation of spatial attention over time. Subjects searched for a target with a unique shape, with a uniquely colored distractor present on each trial. The singleton distractor captured attention early in processing, whereas attention homed in on the target’s location later on. Kim and Cave (1999) concluded that top–down factors cannot prevent the presence of a salient distractor from delaying target selection. The present study tested the idea that such results were obtained only because subjects adopted the strategy of searching for the most salient item. Kim and Cave’s (1999) finding was replicated in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, instead of a feature search, subjects performed a conjunction search—that is, a task that could not be performed using a salience–based strategy. Probe response times were longest at the salient distractor’s location at both the short and the long stimulus onset asynchronies. These results suggest that, early in processing, top–down factors can exert their influence and prevent the capture of attention by a salient distractor.

[1]  J. Hoffman,et al.  Spatial selectivity in visual search , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Y Tsal,et al.  A salient distractor does not disrupt conjunction search , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Steven Todd,et al.  Attentional misguidance in visual search , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  H Pashler,et al.  Cross-dimensional interaction and texture segregation , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  M. R. Houck,et al.  The role of attentional resources in automatic detection , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited , 1990 .

[14]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Charles L. Folk,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998 .

[16]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[18]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  Dominique Lamy,et al.  Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  M. Turatto,et al.  Attentional capture by color without any relevant attentional set , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[23]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Temporal and Spatial Characteristics of Preattentive and Attentive Processing , 1995 .

[24]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  Jeremy M Wolfe,et al.  Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[28]  K. Cave,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  K. Cave,et al.  Spatial Attention in Visual Search for Features and Feature Conjunctions , 1995 .