Recall of early experience and the repressive coping style.

We investigated recall of early experience and the repressive coping style by testing hypotheses concerning restricted access to negative childhood memories and eliciting accounts of quality of parenting received. Repressors free recalled fewer negative childhood memories than nonrepressors, and the age of first negative memory for repressors was older both in free recall and cued recall. Repressors' also took longer to retrieve negative childhood memories but not positive memories. Repressors' accounts of their childhoods were more likely to be characterized by paternal antipathy and indifference, and they were less likely to report an emotionally or physically close relationship with their fathers.

[1]  R. Kobak,et al.  Psychophysiology in Attachment Interviews: Converging Evidence for Deactivating Strategies , 1992 .

[2]  A. Furnham,et al.  Self-criticism in adulthood and recalled childhood experience. , 1992, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[3]  C. Brewin,et al.  From Freud to cognitive science: a contemporary account of the unconscious. , 1991, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[4]  G. Brown,et al.  Intergenerational links between psychiatric disorder in mothers and daughters: the role of parenting experiences. , 1990, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[5]  A. Furnham,et al.  The colour naming of emotional words , 1989 .

[6]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Repression of emotionally tagged memories: the architecture of less complex emotions. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  M. Jensen,et al.  Psychobiological factors predicting the course of breast cancer. , 1987, Journal of personality.

[8]  A. Bifulco,et al.  Loss of parent in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder: the role of lack of adequate parental care , 1986, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  J. Williams,et al.  Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  K. Scherer,et al.  The discrepant repressor: differentiation between low anxiety, high anxiety, and repression of anxiety by autonomic-facial-verbal patterns of behavior. , 1983, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  D. Marlowe,et al.  The Approval Motive: Studies in Evaluative Dependence , 1980 .

[12]  R. Davidson,et al.  Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. , 1979, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  A. W. Bendig,et al.  The development of a short form of the manifest anxiety scale. , 1956, Journal of consulting psychology.

[14]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Psychopathology and early experience: a reappraisal of retrospective reports. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  T. Newton,et al.  Repressive coping and verbal-autonomic response dissociation: the influence of social context. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  M. Main Cross-Cultural Studies of Attachment Organization: Recent Studies, Changing Methodologies, and the Concept of Conditional Strategies , 1990 .

[17]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Repression and the inaccessibility of affective memories. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  J. Herman,et al.  Recovery and verification of memories of childhood sexual trauma. , 1987 .

[19]  G. Gudjonsson Self-reported emotional disturbance and its relation to electrodermal reactivity, defensiveness and trait anxiety , 1981 .

[20]  S. Freud The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud , 1953 .

[21]  Rachel Bowlby,et al.  Studies in hysteria , 1895 .