Known and novel noun extensions: attention at two levels of abstraction.

Two experiments tested the hypothesis that names direct attention at two levels of abstraction: Known names direct attention to the properties most relevant to the specific category; novel names direct attention to the shape, the property most generally relevant across known object names. English-speaking and Japanese-speaking 3-year-olds were shown a novel object that was named with (a) known nouns referring to things similar in shape or similar in material and color, and (b) novel nouns. Given known nouns, children attended to shape when the name referred to a category organized by shape, but they did not when the name referred to a category organized by other properties. Children generalized novel names by shape. The results are discussed within the debate between shape-based and taxonomic categories.

[1]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[2]  D. Rakison When a Rose Is Just a Rose: The Illusion of Taxonomies in Infant Categorization. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[3]  R. Shepard,et al.  Learning and memorization of classifications. , 1961 .

[4]  C. Avgerou,et al.  Concepts and theories , 1998 .

[5]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[6]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[7]  E. Rosch ON THE INTERNAL STRUCTURE OF PERCEPTUAL AND SEMANTIC CATEGORIES1 , 1973 .

[8]  Philip S. Dale,et al.  Macarthur Communicative Development Inventories , 1992 .

[9]  S. Waxman,et al.  The development of a linkage between count nouns and object categories: evidence from fifteen- to twenty-one-month-old infants. , 1993, Child development.

[10]  L. Cohen,et al.  Infants’ Use of Functional Parts in Basic-like Categorization , 1999 .

[11]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  How children know the relevant properties for generalizing object names , 2002 .

[12]  S. Graham,et al.  The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants' inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. , 2001, Child development.

[13]  Learning to Learn Words : A Cross-Linguistic Study of the Shape and Material Biases , 1999 .

[14]  Eliana Colunga,et al.  Making an ontology: Cross-linguistic evidence. , 2003 .

[15]  Terry Kit-fong Au,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive development , 1996 .

[16]  S. Waxman,et al.  Setters and samoyeds: the emergence of subordinate level categories as a basis for inductive inference in preschool-age children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[17]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  S. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children's overextensions of apple and dog* , 1998 .

[20]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.

[21]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. , 1991, Child development.

[22]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15-20-month-olds , 2002 .

[23]  M J Tarr,et al.  Perceptual categorization of cat and dog silhouettes by 3- to 4-month-old infants. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[24]  Nell K. Duke,et al.  Two-year-olds will name artifacts by their functions. , 2000, Child development.

[25]  W. Bruce Croft,et al.  Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children's overextensions of apple and dog , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  L. D. Williams,et al.  Preschoolers' and adults' reliance on object shape and object function for lexical extension. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  J. Mandler,et al.  Studies in Inductive Inference in Infancy , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Shape and the first hundred nouns. , 2004, Child development.

[29]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object Shape, Object Function, and Object Name , 1998 .

[30]  Mark W. Altom,et al.  Correlated symptoms and simulated medical classification. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15-20-month-olds. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[32]  R. Gelman,et al.  Preschooler's Ability to Decide Whether a Photographed Unfamiliar Object Can Move Itself. , 1988 .

[33]  S. Waxman,et al.  Challenging the notion of a thematic preference in young children. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[34]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Reuniting perception and conception , 1998, Cognition.

[35]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Perceptual Similarity and Conceptual Structure , 1992 .

[36]  S. Gelman Chapter 4 – Concepts and Theories , 1996 .

[37]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Children's attention to rigid and deformable shape in naming and non-naming tasks. , 2000, Child development.

[38]  D. Gentner,et al.  Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  How much does a shared name make things similar? Linguistic labels, similarity, and the development of inductive inference. , 2001, Child development.

[40]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category structure and syntax correspond? , 1999, Cognition.

[41]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Children's Theories of Word Meaning: The Role of Shape Similarity in Early Acquisition , 1994 .

[42]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding what to name it , 2000, Cognition.

[43]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[44]  Nathaniel J. Blair,et al.  Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.