Velocity of motion across the skin influences perception of tactile location.

We investigated the influence of motion context on tactile localization, using a paradigm similar to the cutaneous rabbit or sensory saltation (Geldard FA, Sherrick CE. Science 178: 178-179, 1972). In one of its forms, the rabbit stimulus consists of a tap in one location quickly followed by another tap elsewhere, creating the illusion that the two taps are near each other. Instead of taps, we used position of a halted brush and instead of distance judgment, localization responses. The brush moved across the skin of the left forearm, creating a clear motion signal before and after a rabbitlike leap of 10 cm (at 100 cm/s). Three before-and-after velocities (7.5, 15, or 30 cm/s) were used. Participants (n = 13) pointed with their right arm at the felt location of the brush when it halted either 1 cm before or after the leap. These stops were 12 cm apart, but distances computed from localization responses were only 5.4, 6.5, and 7.5 cm for the three velocities, respectively (F[2,11] = 15.19, P = 0.001). Thus the leap resulted in compressive position shift, as described previously for sensory saltation, but modulated by motion velocity before the leap: the slower the motion, the greater the shift opposite to motion direction. No gap in stimulation was perceived. We propose that velocity extrapolation causes the position shift: extrapolated motion does not have enough time to bridge the real spatial gap and thus assigns a closer location to the skin on the opposite side of the gap.

[1]  M. Pettet,et al.  Dynamic changes in receptive-field size in cat primary visual cortex. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Lars Muckli,et al.  Primary Visual Cortex Activity along the Apparent-Motion Trace Reflects Illusory Perception , 2005, PLoS biology.

[3]  F. A. Geldard,et al.  The Cutaneous "Rabbit": A Perceptual Illusion , 1972, Science.

[4]  G. Essick,et al.  Dependence of subjective traverse length on velocity of moving tactile stimuli. , 1986, Somatosensory research.

[5]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.

[6]  H. Helson THE TAU EFFECT--AN EXAMPLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RELATIVITY. , 1931, Science.

[7]  C. Collyer Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments and of timing on spatial judgments , 1977 .

[8]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[9]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[10]  M. Kilgard,et al.  Anticipated stimuli across skin , 1995, Nature.

[11]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Dynamical evolution of motion perception , 2007, Vision Research.

[12]  Terrence J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[13]  F A Geldard,et al.  The saltatory effect in vision. , 1976, Sensory processes.

[14]  Bill Jones,et al.  Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. , 1982 .

[15]  C. van Leeuwen,et al.  Perceptual preferences in depth stratification of transparent layers: Photometric and non-photometric factors. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[16]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Seeing Two as One: Linking Apparent Motion and Repetition Blindness , 1997 .

[17]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[18]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Tracking the apparent location of targets in interpolated motion , 2000, Vision Research.

[19]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  G Westheimer,et al.  A quantitative measure for short-term cortical plasticity in human vision , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  T. Hubbard Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  Shinichi Kita,et al.  Our own faces: perceiving fluctuating asymmetry in the highly familiar objects , 2010 .

[23]  Pierre Kornprobst,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Motion Detection, Integration, and Segregation: From Biology to Artificial Image Processing Systems , 2011, EURASIP J. Adv. Signal Process..

[24]  Tatjana Seizova-Cajic,et al.  Somatosensory Space Abridged: Rapid Change in Tactile Localization Using a Motion Stimulus , 2014, PloS one.

[25]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Finding out about filling-in: a guide to perceptual completion for visual science and the philosophy of perception. , 1998, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[28]  Hong Z. Tan,et al.  Spatiotemporal Integration in Somatosensory Perception: Effects of Sensory Saltation on Pointing at Perceived Positions on the Body Surface , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[29]  Y Dan,et al.  Motion-Induced Perceptual Extrapolation of Blurred Visual Targets , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  H. Cline,et al.  Optic flow instructs retinotopic map formation through a spatial to temporal to spatial transformation of visual information , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Frank A. Geldard,et al.  Sensory saltation : metastability in the perceptual world , 1977 .

[32]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  V. S. RAMACHANDKAN,et al.  EXTRAPOLATION OF MOTION PATH IN HUMAN VISUAL PERCEPTION , 2002 .

[34]  Frank Joublin,et al.  Learning cortical topography from spatiotemporal stimuli , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[35]  S. Getzmann,et al.  Exploring auditory saltation using the "reduced-rabbit" paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  P Cavanagh,et al.  The Position of Moving Objects , 2000, Science.

[37]  F A Geldard,et al.  Saltation in somesthesis. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  Heiko Neumann,et al.  Completion Phenomena in Vision: A Computational Approach , 2003 .

[39]  Markus Lappe,et al.  The Position of Moving Objects , 1998, Science.

[40]  Daniel Goldreich,et al.  A Bayesian Perceptual Model Replicates the Cutaneous Rabbit and Other Tactile Spatiotemporal Illusions , 2007, PloS one.

[41]  Sieu K Khuu,et al.  The effect of motion adaptation on the position of elements in the visual saltation illusion. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[42]  What is the Origin of the Gestalt Principles , 2011 .

[43]  R W Cholewiak,et al.  The generation of vibrotactile patterns on a linear array: Influences of body site, time, and presentation mode , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Filling-in: From perceptual completion to cortical reorganization. , 2003 .

[45]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A selection model for motion processing in area MT of primates , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[46]  Jonathan Tong,et al.  Prediction, Postdiction, and Perceptual Length Contraction: A Bayesian Low-Speed Prior Captures the Cutaneous Rabbit and Related Illusions , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[47]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Reorganization of cortical representations of the hand following alterations of skin inputs induced by nerve injury, skin island transfers, and experience. , 1993, Journal of hand therapy : official journal of the American Society of Hand Therapists.

[48]  R. Cholewiak space, and time , 1999 .

[49]  John Cohen,et al.  A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment , 1953, Nature.