Independent coding of target distance and direction in visuo-spatial working memory

The organization of manual reaching movements suggests considerable independence in the initial programming with respect to the direction and the distance of the intended movement. It was hypothesized that short-term memory for a visually-presented location within reaching space, in the absence of other allocentric reference points, might also be represented in a motoric code, showing similar independence in the encoding of direction and distance. This hypothesis was tested in two experiments, using adult human subjects who were required to remember the location of a briefly presented luminous spot. Stimuli were presented in the dark, thus providing purely egocentric spatial information. After the specified delay, subjects were instructed to point to the remembered location. In Exp. 1, temporal decay of location memory was studied, over a range of 4–30 s. The results showed that (a) memory for both the direction and the distance of the visual target location declined over time, at about the same rate for both parameters; however, (b) errors of distance were much greater in the left than in the right hemispace, whereas direction errors showed no such effect; (c) the distance and direction errors were essentially uncorrelated, at all delays. These findings suggest independent representation of these two parameters in working memory. In Exp. 2 the subjects were required to remember the locations of two visual stimuli presented sequentially, one after the other. Only after both stimuli had been presented did the subject receive a signal from the experimenter as to which one was to be pointed to. The results showed that the encoding of a second location selectively interfered with memory for the direction but not for the distance of the to-be-remembered target location. As in Exp. 1, direction and distance errors were again uncorrelated. The results of both experiments indicate that memory for egocentrically-specified visual locations can encode the direction and distance of the target independently. Use of motor-related representation in spatial working memory is thus strongly suggested. The findings are discussed in the context of multiple representations of space in visuo-spatial short-term memory.

[1]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation specificity in spatial memory: what makes a path different from a map of the path? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[3]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[4]  D. Sparks The neural encoding of the location of targets for saccadic eye movements. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Deficits in attention and movement following the removal of postarcuate (area 6) and prearcuate (area 8) cortex in macaque monkeys. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  L. Hedges,et al.  Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.

[7]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements , 1994 .

[9]  R Caminiti,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  E. B. Knauft,et al.  The accuracy of positioning reactions as a function of their direction and extent. , 1948, The American journal of psychology.

[11]  P Conti,et al.  Role of Structured Visual Field and Visual Reafference in Accuracy of Pointing Movements , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Pure Topographical Disorientation: A Definition and Anatomical Basis , 1987, Cortex.

[13]  John G. Seamon,et al.  Double dissociation of spatial and object visual memory: Evidence from selective interference in intact human subjects , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Selective Impairment in Manipulating Arabic Numerals , 1995, Cortex.

[15]  M. Smyth,et al.  Movement and Working Memory: Patterns and Positions in Space , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Categorical versus coordinate spatial relations: computational analyses and computer simulations. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979 .

[19]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  R. Logie,et al.  Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial, or central executive? , 1991 .

[21]  J. Paillard Motor and representational framing of space , 1991 .

[22]  J. Hummel,et al.  An architecture for rapid, hierarchical structural description , 1996 .

[23]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[24]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  M. Smyth,et al.  Space and Movement in Working Memory , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  M. Woodin,et al.  Skilled Motor Performance and Working Memory in Rowers: Body Patterns and Spatial Positions , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[27]  J. Hellige,et al.  Categorization versus distance: Hemispheric differences for processing spatial information , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[28]  E. Rolls,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric spatial information processing in the hippocampal formation of the behaving primate , 1991, Psychobiology.

[29]  A. Allport,et al.  Independent reference frames in human spatial memory: Body-centered and environmentcentered coding in near and far space , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[30]  K. Kurata,et al.  Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movement-related activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  E. D. de Haan,et al.  What Was Where? Memory for Object Locations , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[32]  M. D. Crutcher,et al.  Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.