Modality shift effects mimic multisensory interactions: an event-related potential study

A frequent approach to study interactions of the auditory and the visual system is to measure event-related potentials (ERPs) to auditory, visual, and auditory-visual stimuli (A, V, AV). A nonzero result of the AV − (A + V) comparison indicates that the sensory systems interact at a specific processing stage. Two possible biases weaken the conclusions drawn by this approach: first, subtracting two ERPs from one requires that A, V, and AV do not share any common activity. We have shown before (Gondan and Röder in Brain Res 1073–1074:389–397, 2006) that the problem of common activity can be avoided using an additional tactile stimulus (T) and evaluating the ERP difference (T + TAV) − (TA + TV). A second possible confound is the modality shift effect (MSE): for example, the auditory N1 is increased if an auditory stimulus follows a visual stimulus, whereas it is smaller if the modality is unchanged (ipsimodal stimulus). Bimodal stimuli might be affected less by MSEs because at least one component always matches the preceding trial. Consequently, an apparent amplitude modulation of the N1 would be observed in AV. We tested the influence of MSEs on auditory-visual interactions by comparing the results of AV − (A + V) using (a) all stimuli and using (b) only ipsimodal stimuli. (a) and (b) differed around 150 ms, this indicates that AV − (A + V) is indeed affected by the MSE. We then formally and empirically demonstrate that (T + TAV) − (TA + TV) is robust against possible biases due to the MSE.

[1]  S. Sterbing-D’Angelo,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Multisensory Space Representations in the Macaque Ventral Intraparietal Area , 2022 .

[2]  S. Mannuzza Cross-modal reaction time and schizophrenic attentional deficit: a critical review. , 1980, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[3]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[4]  B. Argall,et al.  Integration of Auditory and Visual Information about Objects in Superior Temporal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[5]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Human–simian correspondence in the early cortical processing of multisensory cues , 2004, Cognitive Processing.

[6]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  The redundant target effect is affected by modality switch costs , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human posterior parietal and temporoparietal cortices during audiotactile interaction , 2003, NeuroImage.

[8]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  C. Spence,et al.  The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  W. Schwarz,et al.  Diffusion, superposition, and the redundant-targets effect , 1994 .

[11]  H. Colonius Possibly dependent probability summation of reaction time , 1990 .

[12]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory visual-auditory object recognition in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  C. Lammertmann,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic Correlates of Audiotactile Interaction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[14]  D. Raab DIVISION OF PSYCHOLOGY: STATISTICAL FACILITATION OF SIMPLE REACTION TIMES* , 1962 .

[15]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  S. Iversen,et al.  Detection of Audio-Visual Integration Sites in Humans by Application of Electrophysiological Criteria to the BOLD Effect , 2001, NeuroImage.

[17]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Current trends in event-related potential research , 1987 .

[18]  R. Blair,et al.  An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[19]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention , 1993 .

[21]  J. Pernier,et al.  Early auditory-visual interactions in human cortex during nonredundant target identification. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  Rodrigo Quian Quiroga,et al.  Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of intersensory components in audiovisually evoked potentials. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Activation of auditory cortex during silent lipreading. , 1997, Science.

[24]  D. Vorberg,et al.  Distribution inequalities for parallel models with unlimited capacity , 1994 .

[25]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Multisensory convergence at human temporo-parietal junction – epicortical recording of evoked responses , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  A new method for detecting interactions between the senses in event-related potentials , 2006, Brain Research.

[27]  R. Hanewinkel,et al.  Is the modality-shift effect specific for schizophrenia patients? , 1994, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[28]  R. Campbell,et al.  Audiovisual Integration of Speech Falters under High Attention Demands , 2005, Current Biology.

[29]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Grabbing your ear: rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[31]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory auditory-somatosensory interactions in early cortical processing revealed by high-density electrical mapping. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[32]  Alexandra Fort,et al.  Interest and validity of the additive model in electrophysiological studies of multisensory interactions , 2004, Cognitive Processing.

[33]  D. Raab Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times. , 1962, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Juliana Yordanova,et al.  Spatial coincidence modulates interaction between visual and somatosensory evoked potentials , 2002, Neuroreport.

[35]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The timing and laminar profile of converging inputs to multisensory areas of the macaque neocortex. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  The Modality Shift Effect. Further Explorations at the Crossroads , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[37]  J. Lerner,et al.  [The problem of attention in schizophrenia]. , 1973, Harefuah.

[38]  M. Giard,et al.  Auditory-Visual Integration during Multimodal Object Recognition in Humans: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Wallace,et al.  Sensory and Multisensory Responses in the Newborn Monkey Superior Colliculus , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Meyer,et al.  Attention and Performance XIV , 1973 .

[41]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Simple reaction time and statistical facilitation: A parallel grains model , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  C. K. Peck Visual-auditory interactions in cat superior colliculus: their role in the control of gaze , 1987, Brain Research.

[43]  B. Stein,et al.  Spatial factors determine the activity of multisensory neurons in cat superior colliculus , 1986, Brain Research.

[44]  S A Hillyard,et al.  An analysis of audio-visual crossmodal integration by means of event-related potential (ERP) recordings. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  F. Rist,et al.  Effects of modality shift on event-related potentials and reaction time of chronic schizophrenics. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[46]  Joseph Zubin,et al.  Experimental approaches to psychopathology , 1976 .

[47]  Michael C Corballis,et al.  Hemispheric interactions in simple reaction time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Joost X. Maier,et al.  Multisensory Integration of Dynamic Faces and Voices in Rhesus Monkey Auditory Cortex , 2005 .

[49]  M. Wallace,et al.  A revised view of sensory cortical parcellation , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Polysensory properties of neurons in the anterior bank of the caudal superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  A. Diederich,et al.  Bimodal and trimodal multisensory enhancement: Effects of stimulus onset and intensity on reaction time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[52]  D. Barth,et al.  The spatiotemporal organization of auditory, visual, and auditory-visual evoked potentials in rat cortex , 1995, Brain Research.

[53]  Marty G. Woldorff,et al.  Selective Attention and Multisensory Integration: Multiple Phases of Effects on the Evoked Brain Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Audio-visual multisensory integration in superior parietal lobule revealed by human intracranial recordings. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[56]  F. Post,et al.  Behavior, aging and the nervous system , 1966 .

[57]  David Poeppel,et al.  Visual speech speeds up the neural processing of auditory speech. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[59]  Jonathan Ball,et al.  Statistics review 5: Comparison of means , 2002, Critical care.

[60]  Uri Feintuch,et al.  Visual attention and coactivation of response decisions for features from different dimensions. , 2002, Psychological science.

[61]  R. Hare,et al.  The contingent negative variation in psychopaths. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[62]  O. Mowrer,et al.  Preparatory set (expectancy)—an experimental demonstration of its 'central' locus , 1940 .

[63]  J. Pernier,et al.  Dynamics of cortico-subcortical cross-modal operations involved in audio-visual object detection in humans. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.