Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis.

Many recent studies have examined the association between number acuity, which is the ability to rapidly and non-symbolically estimate the quantity of items appearing in a scene, and symbolic math performance. However, various contradictory results have been reported. To comprehensively evaluate the association between number acuity and symbolic math performance, we conduct a meta-analysis to synthesize the results observed in previous studies. First, a meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies (36 samples, N = 4705) revealed a significant positive correlation between these skills (r = 0.20, 95% CI = [0.14, 0.26]); the association remained after considering other potential moderators (e.g., whether general cognitive abilities were controlled). Moreover, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies revealed 1) that number acuity may prospectively predict later math performance (r = 0.24, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.37]; 6 samples) and 2) that number acuity is retrospectively correlated to early math performance as well (r = 0.17, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.26]; 5 samples). In summary, these pieces of evidence demonstrate a moderate but statistically significant association between number acuity and math performance. Based on the estimated effect sizes, power analyses were conducted, which suggested that many previous studies were underpowered due to small sample sizes. This may account for the disparity between findings in the literature, at least in part. Finally, the theoretical and practical implications of our meta-analytic findings are presented, and future research questions are discussed.

[1]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[2]  Wolfgang Viechtbauer,et al.  Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package , 2010 .

[3]  Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al.  Early Numerical Development and the Role of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Skills. , 2013 .

[4]  A. Desoete,et al.  Language in the prediction of arithmetics in kindergarten and grade 1 , 2013 .

[5]  C. Gilmore,et al.  Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[7]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  A sensorimotor paradigm for Bayesian model selection , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[10]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction , 2007, Nature.

[11]  Christian Agrillo,et al.  Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[12]  R. Kirk Practical Significance: A Concept Whose Time Has Come , 1996 .

[13]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.

[14]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  Janosch Linkersdörfer,et al.  Symbolic and non-symbolic distance effects in children and their connection with arithmetic skills , 2011, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[17]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence , 2011, Cognition.

[18]  Stefan Ufer,et al.  How Training on Exact or Approximate Mental Representations of Number Can Enhance First-Grade Students' Basic Number Processing and Arithmetic Skills , 2013 .

[19]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[20]  N. Laird,et al.  Meta-analysis in clinical trials. , 1986, Controlled clinical trials.

[21]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[22]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[23]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Differences between literates and illiterates on symbolic but not nonsymbolic numerical magnitude processing , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[26]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[27]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Preschoolers' Precision of the Approximate Number System Predicts Later School Mathematics Performance , 2011, PloS one.

[28]  A. Desoete,et al.  Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. , 2012, The British journal of educational psychology.

[29]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.

[30]  David C Burr,et al.  Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  Explaining school mathematics performance from symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing: similarities and differences between typical and low-achieving children , 2012 .

[32]  Nicole M. McNeil,et al.  ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. , 2013, Developmental science.

[33]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[34]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense , 2012, PloS one.

[35]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[36]  J. Wilmer How to use individual differences to isolate functional organization, biology, and utility of visual functions; with illustrative proposals for stereopsis. , 2008, Spatial Vision.

[37]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency , 2013, Psychological science.

[38]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The role of reference points in ordinal numerical comparisons by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[39]  J. Leybaert,et al.  Relationships between approximate number system acuity and early symbolic number abilities , 2012, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[40]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[41]  Alex J. Sutton,et al.  Methods for Meta-Analysis in Medical Research , 2000 .

[42]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[43]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[45]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Is Approximate Number Precision a Stable Predictor of Math Ability? , 2013, Learning and individual differences.

[47]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  A Two-Minute Paper-and-Pencil Test of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Explains Variability in Primary School Children's Arithmetic Competence , 2013, PloS one.

[50]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Evidence against a strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent , 2012, CogSci.

[51]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[52]  Jacqueline Leybaert,et al.  Does math education modify the approximate number system? A comparison of schooled and unschooled adults , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[53]  S Duval,et al.  Trim and Fill: A Simple Funnel‐Plot–Based Method of Testing and Adjusting for Publication Bias in Meta‐Analysis , 2000, Biometrics.

[54]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Education Enhances the Acuity of the Nonverbal Approximate Number System , 2013, Psychological science.

[55]  I. Deary,et al.  Intelligence and educational achievement , 2007 .

[56]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers , 2005, Cognition.

[57]  Xin Ma A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ANXIETY TOWARD MATHEMATICS AND ACHIEVEMENT IN MATHEMATICS , 1999 .

[58]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[59]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[60]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.

[61]  Benton J. Underwood,et al.  Individual differences as a crucible in theory construction. , 1975 .

[62]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[63]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[64]  Marcus Lindskog,et al.  Measuring acuity of the approximate number system reliably and validly: the evaluation of an adaptive test procedure , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[65]  S. Varma,et al.  Dyscalculia: From Brain to Education , 2011, Science.

[66]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[67]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Number Sense and Dyscalculia 1 To appear in : Human Behavior , Learning , and the Developing Brain : Atypical Development , 2007 .

[68]  Xinlin Zhou,et al.  Cognitive correlates of performance in advanced mathematics. , 2012, The British journal of educational psychology.

[69]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Links Between the Intuitive Sense of Number and Formal Mathematics Ability. , 2013, Child development perspectives.

[70]  Xitao Fan,et al.  Parental Involvement and Students' Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis , 2001 .

[71]  Xin Ma,et al.  Assessing the Relationship between Attitude toward Mathematics and Achievement in Mathematics: A Meta-Analysis. , 1997 .

[72]  Selcuk R. Sirin Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review of Research , 2005 .

[73]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[74]  Neil Marlow,et al.  Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.

[75]  Hui Zhao,et al.  Gender Differences in Children’s Arithmetic Performance Are Accounted for by Gender Differences in Language Abilities , 2012, Psychological science.

[76]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[77]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.