Inducing abstract linguistic representations: human and connectionist learning of noun classes

Noun class information is a crucial component of the interface between the lexicon and the grammar. In order to explain linguistic productivity it is necessary to assume that linguistic rules are defined not over specific words, but classes of word. This is not only true given the classical distinction between lexicon and grammar, but also in 'emergentist' views which see no clear separation between these two systems (Ellis, 1998; Tomasello, 2000). Even though the latter stress the lexical-specificity of many 'grammatical rules', it is still recognised that adult productivity can only be explained if words are grouped into classes, even if those classes do not map neatly onto traditional linguistic categories. The way in which words are grouped into grammatical classes is therefore an important issue in understanding language development, particularly in explaining the leap from lexical learning to grammar learning. Noun classes, such as grammatical gender, are fundamentally abstract, grammatical, notions (Corbett, 1991). However, attempts have been made to uncover subtle phonological and semantic cues that can be used to predict a word's gender (Kelly, 1992). For example, masculine nouns in German are more likely to be monosyllabic, and monosyllabic words that are masculine contain more consonants than those of other classes. In French, feminine nouns tend to end in closed stressed syllables (e.g. personne, tomate, viande), and masculine nouns tend to end in open stressed syllables (e.g. avion, bruit, chapeau, bain). There are also a number of characteristic derivational morphemes associated with each gender (e.g.-eur and-ment are masculine, and-tion,-euse,-iere are feminine). (Sokolik & Smith, 1992) trained a connectionist network to classify French nouns as either masculine or feminine. The network was presented with the orthographic, rather than phonological, forms of the words. They found that it could then indicate the gender for nouns that it had not received during training, although its performance was 2 not perfect (ranging between 73% and 75%). This indicates that there are regularities in the form (in this case spelling) of French words which can to a certain extent predict gender category. Yet there are always words which fall stubbornly outside such generalisations. In the case of French, Carroll (2001) argues that in any case, the kinds of phonological cues that have been appealed to are more subtle than could reasonably be expected to be represented in the lexicon. This is not to say that phonological and semantic cues do not play a …

[1]  M. Garrett,et al.  Grammatical Gender Is on the Tip of Italian Tongues , 1997 .

[2]  S. C. Howell,et al.  Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Child Development , 2003 .

[3]  L. Squire,et al.  Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  M. Tomasello The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Susanne Elizabeth Carroll,et al.  Input and Evidence: The raw material of second language acquisition , 2001 .

[6]  Peter Lovatt,et al.  Phonological memory and rule learning , 2003 .

[7]  R. Mathews,et al.  Role of Implicit and Explicit Processes in Learning From Examples: A Synergistic Effect , 1989 .

[8]  M. Maratsos,et al.  The internal language of children's syntax : The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories , 1980 .

[9]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[10]  L B Leonard,et al.  Toward mastery of Italian morphology: a cross-sectional study , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  Susanne Carroll,et al.  Second-Language Acquisition and the Computational Paradigm* , 1989 .

[12]  R. Mathews,et al.  Role of Implicit and Explicit Processes in Learning From Examples : A Synergistic Effect , 2004 .

[13]  R. Gómez,et al.  Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Michael T. Ullman,et al.  The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: the declarative/procedural model , 2001 .

[15]  R. French,et al.  Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective , 2002 .

[16]  Gary F Marcus,et al.  Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: can simple recurrent networks obviate the need for domain-specific learning devices? , 1999, Cognition.

[17]  Lydia White,et al.  The Second Language Acquisition of Spanish DPS: the Status of Grammatical Features , 2002 .

[18]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[20]  Patricia J. Brooks,et al.  Exploring language acquisition in children with a miniature artificial language: Effects of item and pattern frequency, arbitrary subclasses, and correction☆ , 1990 .

[21]  R. Hawkins Second Language Syntax: A Generative Introduction , 2001 .

[22]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[23]  J. Saffran The Use of Predictive Dependencies in Language Learning , 2001 .

[24]  Roman Taraban,et al.  Gender processing in native and nonnative Russian speakers , 1999, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[25]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[26]  Michael Smith,et al.  Assignment of gender to French nouns in primary and secondary language : a connectionist model , 1992 .

[27]  Ron Sun,et al.  From implicit skills to explicit knowledge: a bottom-up model of skill learning , 2001, Cogn. Sci..

[28]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. , 1992, Psychological review.

[29]  N. Ellis Emergentism, Connectionism, and Language Learning. , 1998 .

[30]  Nick Chater,et al.  Connectionist and Statistical Approaches to Language Acquisition: A Distributional Perspective. , 1998 .

[31]  Lydia White,et al.  L2 Acquisition of Spanish DPs: the status of grammatical features. , 2000 .

[32]  Roger Hawkins,et al.  Approaches to Second Language Acquisition , 1994 .

[33]  Janet L. McDonald,et al.  Properties of Phonological Markers That Affect the Acquisition of Gender-Like Subclasses☆☆☆★ , 1998 .

[34]  David R. Shanks,et al.  The Psychology of Associative Learning , 1995 .

[35]  D. Shanks,et al.  Two mechanisms in implicit artificial grammar learning? Comment on Meulemans and Van der Linden (1997). , 1999 .

[36]  Patricia J. Brooks,et al.  Acquisition of Gender-like Noun Subclasses in an Artificial Language: The Contribution of Phonological Markers to Learning , 1993 .

[37]  Zenon W. Pylyshyn,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[38]  P. Skehan 语言学习认知法 = A cognitive approach to language learning , 1998 .

[39]  Susanne Carroll,et al.  Input and SLA: Adults' Sensitivity to Different Sorts of Cues to French Gender , 1999 .

[40]  Thierry Meulemans,et al.  Associative chunk strength in artificial grammar learning , 1997 .

[41]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Exercises in Rethinking Innateness: A Handbook for Connectionist Simulations , 1997 .

[42]  G. Marcus,et al.  The scope of linguistic generalizations: evidence from Hebrew word formation , 2002, Cognition.