Foetal and postnatal head growth and risk of cognitive decline in old age.

Studies of elderly people have shown that scores on tests of cognitive function tend to be higher in those with larger head circumferences. One explanation for these findings is that optimal brain development in utero and in the first years of life may protect against cognitive decline in old age, though the relative importance of these two periods of brain growth is unclear. We assessed change in cognitive function over a 3.5-year period in 215 men and women aged 66-75 years whose head circumference had been recorded at birth and as adults. Cognitive function was tested in the initial study and at follow-up with the AH4 intelligence test and the Wechsler Logical Memory test. We found no associations between head circumference at birth and score on the cognitive function tests or change in score over time. However, people who had a larger head circumference as an adult gained significantly higher scores on the intelligence test on both testing occasions and were less likely to show a decline in memory performance over the follow-up period. People whose head circumference was in the top quarter of the distribution had an odds ratio for decline in immediate recall on the Logical Memory test of 0.2 (95% confidence interval 0.1-0.6) and an odds ratio for decline in delayed recall of 0.3 (95% confidence interval 0.1-0.9) compared with those whose head circumference was in the bottom quarter, after adjustment for age, sex and potential risk factors. These results suggest that brain development during infancy and early childhood is important in determining how well cognitive abilities are preserved in old age.

[1]  L. Cohen,et al.  The Molecular Basis of Hypopituitarism , 1998, Hormone Research in Paediatrics.

[2]  F. Johnston,et al.  The Cambridge encyclopedia of human growth and development , 1998 .

[3]  P. Yudkin,et al.  Head circumference as an index of brain weight in the fetus and newborn. , 1977, Early human development.

[4]  F. Nouri,et al.  Use of the Nottingham Health Profile with patients after a stroke. , 1986, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[5]  Karen J. Ferguson,et al.  Intracranial capacity and brain volumes are associated with cognition in healthy elderly men , 2002, Neurology.

[6]  M N Rossor,et al.  Intracranial volume and Alzheimer disease: evidence against the cerebral reserve hypothesis. , 2000, Archives of neurology.

[7]  E M Backett,et al.  A quantitative approach to perceived health status: a validation study. , 1980, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[8]  D. Deeg Attrition in longitudinal population studies: Does it affect the generalizability of the findings?: An introduction to the series☆ , 2002 .

[9]  Donald H. Lee,et al.  Relationships between factors of intelligence and brain volume , 2000 .

[10]  J. Jolles,et al.  Predictors of attrition in a longitudinal cognitive aging study: the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS). , 2002, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[11]  Ian J. Deary,et al.  The Stability of Individual Differences in Mental Ability from Childhood to Old Age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey , 2000 .

[12]  Y. Stern What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[13]  R. Mayeux,et al.  The age at onset of Alzheimer's disease and an intracranial area measurement. A relationship. , 1995, Archives of neurology.

[14]  Y. Stern,et al.  An association between head circumference and Alzheimer's disease in a population-based study of aging and dementia , 1997, Neurology.

[15]  David Cecil Sinclair,et al.  Human Growth After Birth , 1973 .

[16]  G. Belle,et al.  Cholinergic dysfunction in diseases with Lewy bodies , 2000, Neurology.

[17]  G. Schellenberg,et al.  Head circumference and incident Alzheimer’s disease , 2001, Neurology.

[18]  S. DeKosky,et al.  Small head size is related to low Mini-Mental State Examination scores in a community sample of nondemented older adults , 1999, Neurology.

[19]  F. Norris Effects of attrition on relationships between variables in surveys of older adults. , 1987, Journal of gerontology.

[20]  Experience with Growth Hormone Therapy in Turner Syndrome in a Single Centre: Low Total Height Gain, No Further Gains after Puberty Onset and Unchanged Body Proportions , 2000, Hormone Research in Paediatrics.

[21]  I. Steiner,et al.  Head size and cognitive ability in nondemented older adults are related. , 2001 .

[22]  C. Martyn,et al.  Growth in utero and cognitive function in adult life: follow up study of people born between 1920 and 1943 , 1996, BMJ.

[23]  E G Tangalos,et al.  Total intracranial volume: Normative values and lack of association with Alzheimer’s disease , 2002, Neurology.

[24]  H. Syddall,et al.  Relation between size at birth and risk of age-related macular degeneration. , 2002, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[25]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Wechsler memory scale-revised , 1988 .