The Influence of Subjective Alertness and Motivation on Human Performance Independent of Circadian and Homeostatic Regulation

Endogenous circadian rhythmicity and sleep-wake homeostasis are robust regulators of human alertness and performance, yet few studies have examined how these regulatory processes affect motivation. Moreover, the influence of alertness and motivation on performance, independent of circadian phase and hours awake, has not been studied. Healthy subjects, 12 males and 3 females, ages 20 to 41, participated in a 2-week 28-h forced desynchrony protocol to address these issues. Subjects performed a battery of tests every 2 hours during scheduled wakefulness. Performance on a mathematical addition test and ratings of alertness and motivation on visual analog scales were analyzed. Performance scores were categorized as being associated with the highest or lowest alertness and motivation ratings for each circadian phase/hours awake bin to determine whether high levels of alertness and motivation resulted in higher performance scores above and beyond the effects of circadian and homeostatic regulation. Motivation varied significantly as a function of circadian phase and hours awake. Motivation and alertness were correlated. When circadian phase and hours awake were accounted for, performance was better when alertness and motivation ratings were highest and worse when those ratings were lowest. The present findings suggest that human performance is influenced by alertness and motivation independent of circadian phase and hours awake. Future studies examining the influence of circadian phase and sleep-wake homeostasis on human performance also should assess alertness and motivation to aid in the interpretation of performance data. Such studies also may aid in the development of countermeasures to improve human performance.

[1]  Ronald Seifer,et al.  Intrinsic circadian period of adolescent humans measured in conditions of forced desynchrony , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[2]  A. Pettitt,et al.  High incentive effects on vigilance performance during 72 hours of total sleep deprivation. , 1985, Acta psychologica.

[3]  T H Monk,et al.  Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[4]  D. Dijk,et al.  Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms , sleep , and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20h day , 1999 .

[5]  R. R. Mackie,et al.  Vigilance: Theory, Operational Performance, and Physiological Correlates , 1978 .

[6]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. , 2002, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[7]  P. Badia,et al.  Effects of menstrual cycle phase and oral contraceptives on alertness, cognitive performance, and circadian rhythms during sleep deprivation , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  R. Ogilvie,et al.  Sleep onset: Normal and abnormal processes. , 1994 .

[9]  Haslam Dr The incentive effect and sleep deprivation. , 1983 .

[10]  O. Edholm,et al.  THE PHYSIOLOGY OF HUMAN SURVIVAL , 1965 .

[11]  P. Badia,et al.  Combination of bright light and caffeine as a countermeasure for impaired alertness and performance during extended sleep deprivation , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[12]  Anders Kjellberg,et al.  Sleep Deprivation, Arousal, and Performance , 1977 .

[13]  Torbjörn Åkerstedt,et al.  Melatonin excretion, body temperature and subjective arousal during 64 hours of sleep deprivation , 1979, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  D. Dawson,et al.  When the human circadian system is caught napping: evidence for endogenous rhythms close to 24 hours. , 1993, Sleep.

[15]  Monk Th,et al.  The arousal model of time of day effects in human performance efficiency. , 1982 .

[16]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Entrained phase of the circadian pacemaker serves to stabilize alertness and performance throughout the habitual waking day. , 1994 .

[17]  A. Wing,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on short duration performance measures compared to the Wilkinson auditory vigilance task. , 1978, Sleep.

[18]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Intrinsic near-24-h pacemaker period determines limits of circadian entrainment to a weak synchronizer in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Kinney,et al.  Physiology, stress, and malnutrition : functional correlates, nutritional intervention , 1997 .

[20]  A. Gaillard,et al.  The effects of sleep deprivation and incentives on human performance , 1993, Psychological research.

[21]  N. Kleitman,et al.  Sleep and Wakefulness , 1965 .

[22]  A. Borbély Processes Underlying Sleep Regulation , 1998, Hormone Research in Paediatrics.

[23]  C. Karlsson,et al.  CIRCADIAN VARIATIONS IN PERFORMANCE, PSYCHOLOGICAL RATINGS, CATECHOLAMINE EXCRETION AND URINE FLOW DURING PROLONGED SLEEP DEPRIVATION , 1970 .

[24]  D. Dinges,et al.  Performing while sleepy: Effects of experimentally-induced sleepiness. , 1991 .

[25]  D.G.M. Dijk,et al.  Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. , 1999, Science.

[27]  T. Monk Sleep, sleepiness, and performance , 1991 .

[28]  A. Daurat,et al.  Bright light affects alertness and performance rhythms during a 24-h constant routine , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[29]  Fred W. Turek,et al.  Regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms , 1999 .

[30]  Charles A. Czeisler,et al.  INFLUENCE OF LIGHT ON CIRCADIAN RHYTHMICITY IN HUMANS , 1999 .

[31]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[32]  B M Stone,et al.  Human circadian rhythms in constant dim light (8 lux) with knowledge of clock time , 1996, Journal of sleep research.

[33]  R. H. van den Hoofdakker,et al.  Endogenous and exogenous components in the circadian variation of core body temperature in humans , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[34]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Short‐term memory, alertness and performance: a reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[35]  J. Horne Why we sleep , 1988 .

[36]  T. Reilly,et al.  Lack of evidence that feedback from lifestyle alters the amplitude of the circadian pacemaker in humans. , 1999, Chronobiology international.

[37]  W. Colquhoun Circadian rythms, mental efficiency and shift work. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[38]  E. Deci,et al.  Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions. , 2000, Contemporary educational psychology.

[39]  D. Dijk,et al.  Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[40]  D. Dijk,et al.  Later endogenous circadian temperature nadir relative to an earlier wake time in older people. , 1998, The American journal of physiology.

[41]  R. Wilkinson,et al.  Interaction of alcohol with incentive and with sleep deprivation. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  S Folkard,et al.  Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy subjects. , 1997, Archives of general psychiatry.