Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

During the retention of visual information in working memory, event-related brain potentials show a sustained negativity over posterior visual regions contralateral to the side where memorized stimuli were presented. This contralateral delay activity (CDA) is generally believed to be a neural marker of working memory storage. In two experiments, we contrasted this storage account of the CDA with the alternative hypothesis that the CDA reflects the current focus of spatial attention on a subset of memorized items set up during the most recent encoding episode. We employed a sequential loading procedure where participants memorized four task-relevant items that were presented in two successive memory displays (M1 and M2). In both experiments, CDA components were initially elicited contralateral to task-relevant items in M1. Critically, the CDA switched polarity when M2 displays appeared on the opposite side. In line with the attentional activation account, these reversed CDA components exclusively reflected the number of items that were encoded from M2 displays, irrespective of how many M1 items were already held in working memory. On trials where M1 and M2 displays were presented on the same side and on trials where M2 displays appeared nonlaterally, CDA components elicited in the interval after M2 remained sensitive to a residual trace of M1 items, indicating that some activation of previously stored items was maintained across encoding episodes. These results challenge the hypothesis that CDA amplitudes directly reflect the total number of stored objects and suggest that the CDA is primarily sensitive to the activation of a subset of working memory representations within the current focus of spatial attention.

[1]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Cody Ding,et al.  Reduced representations capacity in visual working memory in trait anxiety , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[3]  M. Tsodyks,et al.  Synaptic Theory of Working Memory , 2008, Science.

[4]  M. Eimer,et al.  Does visual working memory represent the predicted locations of future target objects? An event-related brain potential study , 2015, Brain Research.

[5]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  Toward the neural mechanisms of reduced working memory capacity in schizophrenia. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Delay-period Activity in the Prefrontal Cortex: One Function Is Sensory Gating , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[8]  K. Oberauer The focus of attention in working memory—from metaphors to mechanisms , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[9]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  No Behavioral or ERP Evidence for a Developmental Lag in Visual Working Memory Capacity or Filtering in Adolescents and Adults with ADHD , 2013, PloS one.

[10]  G. Woodman,et al.  The neurophysiological index of visual working memory maintenance is not due to load dependent eye movements , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Electrophysiological Measures of Maintaining Representations in Visual Working Memory , 2007, Cortex.

[12]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Decoding Attended Information in Short-term Memory: An EEG Study , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  E. Vogel,et al.  Interactions between attention and working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[15]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Inferior Temporal, Prefrontal, and Hippocampal Contributions to Visual Working Memory Maintenance and Associative Memory Retrieval , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Influence of Working-Memory Demand and Subject Performance on Prefrontal Cortical Activity , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Neural Evidence for a Distinction between Short-term Memory and the Focus of Attention , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  E. Vogel,et al.  Neural Measures of Individual Differences in Selecting and Tracking Multiple Moving Objects , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  Edward K Vogel,et al.  Flexibility in visual working memory: Accurate change detection in the face of irrelevant variations in position , 2012, Visual cognition.

[21]  P. Goldman-Rakic Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[22]  Julie D. Golomb,et al.  A taxonomy of external and internal attention. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[23]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Contralateral delay activity reveals life-span age differences in top-down modulation of working memory contents. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[25]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  From cognitive to neural models of working memory , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Dissecting Contributions of Prefrontal Cortex and Fusiform Face Area to Face Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Lateralized Delay Period Activity Marks the Focus of Spatial Attention in Working Memory: Evidence from Somatosensory Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Delineating the Neural Signatures of Tracking Spatial Position and Working Memory during Attentive Tracking , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  F. Tong,et al.  Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas , 2009, Nature.

[30]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[31]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Sensory Recruitment Model of Somatosensory Working Memory. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Neuron Activity Related to Short-Term Memory , 1971, Science.

[33]  Adam C. Riggall,et al.  Distributed Patterns of Activity in Sensory Cortex Reflect the Precision of Multiple Items Maintained in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Kartik K. Sreenivasan,et al.  Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[36]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Immature Processing Capacity and Filtering in Visuospatial Working Memory in Adolescents , 2012, PloS one.

[37]  E. Vogel,et al.  Contralateral delay activity provides a neural measure of the number of representations in visual working memory. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Jarrod A. Lewis-Peacock,et al.  Multiple neural states of representation in short-term memory? It’s a matter of attention , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..