The role of volleyball expertise in motor simulation.

We explored the impact of motor experience on the interaction between implicit motor simulation and language-processing. In an action familiarity judgment task, expert volleyball players, fans and novices were presented with semantically correct sentences describing possible and not possible motor actions, all as negative or positive contexts, e.g., "Don't shank!" or "Assist!". As processing negated action-phrases is known to reduce simulation states, exposure to negative or positive contexts was used here to test how simulation varies according to motor feasibility (possible, impossible) and experience (experts and fans). A significant group×stimulus×context interaction showed that athletes and fans, took longer to process negative than positive contexts for possible actions, compared to action-impossible sentences. In addition, experts were significantly faster and more accurate than fans and, in turn, they were both more accurate than novices. Thus, implicit motor simulation impacts on action-verb processing depending on (i) the domain-relevant expertise, (ii) the feasibility of the actions, and (iii) on whether scenes are presented in a negated context. These results suggest that the implicit triggering of motor representations is modulated by the context and it is tuned to people's motor repertoire, even when actions are described linguistically.

[1]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.

[4]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Suppression of corticospinal excitability during negative motor imagery. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  T. Hammeke,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging mapping of the motor cortex in patients with cerebral tumors. , 1996, Neurosurgery.

[6]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  New aspects of motion perception: selective neural encoding of apparent human movements , 2000, Neuroreport.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Congruent Embodied Representations for Visually Presented Actions and Linguistic Phrases Describing Actions , 2006, Current Biology.

[10]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenous forces: Distinct neural mechanisms , 2001, Neuroreport.

[11]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mental imaging of motor activity in humans , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Stimulus properties matter more than perspective: An fMRI study of mental imagery and silent reading of action phrases , 2007, NeuroImage.

[14]  Andrea Moro,et al.  Negation in the brain: Modulating action representations , 2008, NeuroImage.

[15]  M. Just,et al.  Changes in activation levels with negation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[17]  B. Kaup,et al.  Negation and its impact on the accessibility of text information , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[18]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Barbara Kaup,et al.  Event-related Potential Correlates of Negation in a Sentence-Picture Verification Paradigm , 2008, J. Cogn. Neurosci..

[20]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Experiential simulations of negated text information , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  Gian Luca Romani,et al.  Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an FMRI study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Effects of negation and situational presence on the accessibility of text information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[26]  Katie L. McMahon,et al.  Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices , 2008, NeuroImage.

[27]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Effects of TMS on Different Stages of Motor and Non-Motor Verb Processing in the Primary Motor Cortex , 2009, PloS one.

[30]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  When affordances climb into your mind: Advantages of motor simulation in a memory task performed by novice and expert rock climbers , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Selective Activation of a Parietofrontal Circuit during Implicitly Imagined Prehension , 2002, NeuroImage.

[32]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  P. Weiss,et al.  To move or not to move: imperatives modulate action-related verb processing in the motor system , 2010, Neuroscience.

[34]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[35]  Peter T. Fox,et al.  The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape , 1998 .

[36]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Pickup of Essential Kinematics Underpins Expert Perception of Movement Patterns , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  L. Tyler,et al.  Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words and action sentences , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Comprehending Prehending: Neural Correlates of Processing Verbs with Motor Stems , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Claudia Scorolli,et al.  Language comprehension and dominant hand motion simulation. , 2009, Human movement science.

[40]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: A comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  A. Borghi,et al.  Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Dissociating neural correlates for nouns and verbs , 2005, NeuroImage.

[43]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Do Expertise and the Degree of Perception—Action Coupling Affect Natural Anticipatory Performance? , 2003, Perception.

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  B. Bardy,et al.  Perception-action coupling and expertise in interceptive actions. , 2005, Human movement science.

[47]  A. Borghi,et al.  Sentence comprehension and action: Effector specific modulation of the motor system , 2007, Brain Research.

[48]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Methods for Dealing With Reaction Time Outliers , 1992 .

[49]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Imagined transformations of bodies: an fMRI investigation , 1999, Neuropsychologia.