Spatiotemporal Processing in Crossmodal Interactions for Perception of the External World: A Review

Research regarding crossmodal interactions has garnered much interest in the last few decades. A variety of studies have demonstrated that multisensory information (vision, audition, tactile sensation, and so on) can perceptually interact with each other in the spatial and temporal domains. Findings regarding crossmodal interactions in the spatiotemporal domain (i.e., motion processing) have also been reported, with updates in the last few years. In this review, we summarize past and recent findings on spatiotemporal processing in crossmodal interactions regarding perception of the external world. A traditional view regarding crossmodal interactions holds that vision is superior to audition in spatial processing, but audition is dominant over vision in temporal processing. Similarly, vision is considered to have dominant effects over the other sensory modalities (i.e., visual capture) in spatiotemporal processing. However, recent findings demonstrate that sound could have a driving effect on visual motion perception. Moreover, studies regarding perceptual associative learning reported that, after association is established between a sound sequence without spatial information and visual motion information, the sound sequence could trigger visual motion perception. Other sensory information, such as motor action or smell, has also exhibited similar driving effects on visual motion perception. Additionally, recent brain imaging studies demonstrate that similar activation patterns could be observed in several brain areas, including the motion processing areas, between spatiotemporal information from different sensory modalities. Based on these findings, we suggest that multimodal information could mutually interact in spatiotemporal processing in the percept of the external world and that common perceptual and neural underlying mechanisms would exist for spatiotemporal processing.

[1]  M. Ernst,et al.  Modeling multisensory integration , 2014 .

[2]  G. Essick,et al.  Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Attending to visual or auditory motion affects perception within and across modalities: an event‐related potential study , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  Shuichi Sakamoto,et al.  Auditory Motion Information Drives Visual Motion Perception , 2011, PloS one.

[5]  I H Bernstein,et al.  Effects of some variations in auditory input upon visual choice reaction time. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[7]  Yôiti Suzuki,et al.  Implicit estimation of sound-arrival time , 2003, Nature.

[8]  G. Calvert Crossmodal processing in the human brain: insights from functional neuroimaging studies. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Wataru Teramoto,et al.  Sounds Move a Static Visual Object , 2010, PloS one.

[10]  Julian Keil,et al.  Visuotactile motion congruence enhances gamma-band activity in visual and somatosensory cortices , 2015, NeuroImage.

[11]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory contributions to the perception of motion , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Simon Carlile,et al.  Compression of auditory space during rapid head turns , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[16]  R Fendrich,et al.  The temporal cross-capture of audition and vision , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  C. Spence Just how important is spatial coincidence to multisensory integration? Evaluating the spatial rule , 2013, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  K. Zilles,et al.  Polymodal Motion Processing in Posterior Parietal and Premotor Cortex A Human fMRI Study Strongly Implies Equivalencies between Humans and Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.

[19]  Vision contingent auditory pitch aftereffects , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  D. C. Higgins Human Spatial Orientation , 1967, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[21]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[22]  Shuichi Sakamoto,et al.  Compression of Auditory Space during Forward Self-Motion , 2012, PloS one.

[23]  C. Spence Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[24]  W. Singer,et al.  Capture of Auditory Motion by Vision Is Represented by an Activation Shift from Auditory to Visual Motion Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Mark W Greenlee,et al.  Neural correlates of coherent audiovisual motion perception. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Emiliano Macaluso,et al.  Audio–visual interactions for motion perception in depth modulate activity in visual area V3A , 2013, NeuroImage.

[27]  Ulrik R. Beierholm,et al.  Causal inference in perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Yukio Iwaya,et al.  Visual motion perception induced by sounds in vertical plane , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  M. Murray,et al.  Looming Signals Reveal Synergistic Principles of Multisensory Integration , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  W. Singer,et al.  Auditory motion direction encoding in auditory cortex and high‐level visual cortex , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Benefits of Stimulus Congruency for Multisensory Facilitation of Visual Learning , 2008, PloS one.

[32]  Christoph Kayser,et al.  Oscillatory mechanisms underlying the enhancement of visual motion perception by multisensory congruency , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Jon Driver,et al.  Direction of Visual Apparent Motion Driven Solely by Timing of a Static Sound , 2008, Current Biology.

[34]  M. Ernst Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[35]  Souta Hidaka,et al.  Effect of pitch–space correspondence on sound-induced visual motion perception , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  S. Nishida,et al.  A common perceptual temporal limit of binding synchronous inputs across different sensory attributes and modalities , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  S. Nishida,et al.  Audio–tactile superiority over visuo–tactile and audio–visual combinations in the temporal resolution of synchrony perception , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  N Mai,et al.  Disturbance of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain damage. Further evidence and follow up observations. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Tao Zhang,et al.  Smelling directions: Olfaction modulates ambiguous visual motion perception , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[40]  Yukio Iwaya,et al.  Alternation of Sound Location Induces Visual Motion Perception of a Static Object , 2009, PloS one.

[41]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.

[42]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance , 2012, Current Biology.

[43]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Lukas Scheef,et al.  Multimodal motion processing in area V5/MT: Evidence from an artificial class of audio-visual events , 2009, Brain Research.

[45]  Hulusi Kafaligonul,et al.  Audiovisual associations alter the perception of low-level visual motion , 2015, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[46]  T. Albright,et al.  Remembering Visual Motion: Neural Correlates of Associative Plasticity and Motion Recall in Cortical Area MT , 2007, Neuron.

[47]  G F Meyer,et al.  The integration of auditory and visual motion signals at threshold , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  B. Stein The new handbook of multisensory processes , 2012 .

[49]  D. Angelaki,et al.  Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  Norimichi Kitagawa,et al.  Hearing visual motion in depth , 2002, Nature.

[51]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Illusions: What you see is what you hear , 2000, Nature.

[53]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Preperceptual and Stimulus-Selective Enhancement of Low-Level Human Visual Cortex Excitability by Sounds , 2009, Current Biology.

[54]  Maori Kobayashi,et al.  Indiscriminable sounds determine the direction of visual motion , 2012, Scientific reports.

[55]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Tactile and visual motion direction processing in hMT+/V5 , 2014, NeuroImage.

[56]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[57]  Peter Janssen,et al.  The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in the macaque monkey , 2016 .

[58]  Stephan Getzmann,et al.  The Effect of Brief Auditory Stimuli on Visual Apparent Motion , 2007, Perception.

[59]  Y. Sugita,et al.  Sound-contingent visual motion aftereffect , 2011, BMC Neuroscience.

[60]  M. Ernst,et al.  When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration , 2012, Current Biology.

[61]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Causal Inference in Multisensory Perception , 2007, PloS one.

[62]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Touch can change visual slant perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  E. DeYoe,et al.  A comparison of visual and auditory motion processing in human cerebral cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  Megan A. K. Peters,et al.  0 + 1 > 1 , 2012, Psychological science.

[65]  M. Ernst,et al.  Feeling what you hear: auditory signals can modulate tactile tap perception , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  J. Driver,et al.  Multisensory Interplay Reveals Crossmodal Influences on ‘Sensory-Specific’ Brain Regions, Neural Responses, and Judgments , 2008, Neuron.

[67]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[68]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.

[69]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Assessing automaticity in the audiovisual integration of motion. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[70]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Simultaneous and Independent Acquisition of Multisensory and Unisensory Associations , 2007, Perception.

[71]  Charles Spence,et al.  Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  Charles Spence,et al.  Cross-correlation between auditory and visual signals promotes multisensory integration. , 2013, Multisensory research.

[73]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[74]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[75]  P Bertelson,et al.  Auditory-visual interaction and the timing of inputs , 1987, Psychological research.

[76]  Kenith V. Sobel,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Neural Synergy Between Kinetic Vision and Touch , 2022 .

[77]  Alessio Fracasso,et al.  Fooling the Eyes: The Influence of a Sound-Induced Visual Motion Illusion on Eye Movements , 2013, PloS one.

[78]  Y. Sugita,et al.  Sound Frequency and Aural Selectivity in Sound-Contingent Visual Motion Aftereffect , 2012, PloS one.

[79]  Olga Sachs,et al.  Spatially congruent visual motion modulates activity of the primary auditory cortex , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[80]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Dynamic Visual Capture: Apparent Auditory Motion Induced by a Moving Visual Target , 1985, Perception.

[81]  C. Spence,et al.  International Multisensory Research Forum 2012 meeting special issue. , 2013, Multisensory Research.

[82]  Takahiro Kawabe,et al.  Audiovisual tau effect. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[83]  C. Spence,et al.  Attention and the crossmodal construction of space , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[84]  Mowbray Gh,et al.  On discriminating the rate of visual flicker and auditory flutter. , 1959 .

[85]  Jean Vroomen,et al.  Neural correlates of audiovisual motion capture , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[86]  S. Wuerger,et al.  Cross-modal integration of auditory and visual motion signals , 2001, Neuroreport.

[87]  David Alais,et al.  No direction-specific bimodal facilitation for audiovisual motion detection. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[88]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Changing pitch induced visual motion illusion , 2004, Current Biology.

[89]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Sound Facilitates Visual Learning , 2006, Current Biology.

[90]  M. Rizzo,et al.  Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[91]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  The ventriloquist in motion: illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[92]  Sidney S. Simon,et al.  Merging of the Senses , 2008, Front. Neurosci..

[93]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory interactions in the depth plane in front and rear space: A review , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[94]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Moving Multisensory Research Along , 2004 .

[95]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The motion aftereffect , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[96]  Nan-Suey Liu,et al.  Mach number distribution and plume direction prediction of a rocket thruster operating at four different combustion chamber pressures , 2004, Journal of Vision.

[97]  C. Spence,et al.  The Handbook of Multisensory Processing , 2004 .

[98]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Direction of visual apparent motion driven by perceptual organization of cross-modal signals. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[99]  Atsushi Okajima,et al.  Flow visualization of coaxial jet excited with varying phase differences , 2004, J. Vis..

[100]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Cross-modal dynamic capture: congruency effects in the perception of motion across sensory modalities. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[101]  J. Bizley The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes , 2011 .

[102]  Robert A Jacobs,et al.  Parameter learning but not structure learning: a Bayesian network model of constraints on early perceptual learning. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[103]  J. Saunders,et al.  Demonstration of cue recruitment: change in visual appearance by means of Pavlovian conditioning. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[104]  G. Fink,et al.  REVIEW: The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and monkeys , 2005, Journal of anatomy.

[105]  T. Stanford,et al.  Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[106]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Motion Aftereffects Transfer between Touch and Vision , 2009, Current Biology.

[107]  S. Nishida,et al.  Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[108]  G H MOWBRAY,et al.  On discriminating the rate of visual flicker and auditory flutter. , 1959, The American journal of psychology.

[109]  S. Shimojo,et al.  When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.

[110]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Receptive-field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.