Improving left hemispatial neglect using virtual reality

Background: In hemispatial neglect, space can be dissociated on the basis of “near” peripersonal vs “far” extrapersonal space. The clinical manifestations of neglect can be modified by having patients use a tool to explore “far” extrapersonal space. An explanation for this is that the use of a stick produced an extension of body space resulting in a remapping of “far” space as “near” space. Objectives: To determine whether the remapping of space can be generalized to the amelioration of the “affected” vs the “nonaffected” space, rather than being confined to the selective amelioration of “far” vs “near” neglect; and to determine whether tool use is a necessary condition for the remapping of space. Methods: Using virtual reality, the authors asked six hemispatial neglect patients to reach and grasp a real object while simultaneously observing the grasping of a virtual object located within a virtual environment by a virtual hand. The virtual hand was commanded in real time by their real hand. Results: After a period of adaptation, hemispatial neglect patients coded the visual stimuli within the neglected space in an identical fashion as those presented within the preserved portions of space. Conclusions: It is possible to re-create links between the affected and the nonaffected space. Wielding a tool is not a necessary condition in reopening neglected space.

[1]  Jon Driver,et al.  Active Tool Use with the Contralesional Hand Can Reduce Cross-modal Extinction of Touch on that Hand , 2002, Neurocase.

[2]  F. Meneghello,et al.  Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  Widening the Sphere of Influence: Using a Tool to Extend Extrapersonal Visual Space in a Patient with Severe Neglect , 2002, Neurocase.

[4]  T Landis,et al.  So near yet so far: Neglect in far or near space depends on tool use , 2001, Annals of neurology.

[5]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Self-images in the video monitor coded by monkey intraparietal neurons , 2001, Neuroscience Research.

[6]  A Farnè,et al.  Dynamic size‐change of hand peripersonal space following tool use , 2000, Neuroreport.

[7]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[9]  Jeremy D Schmahmann,et al.  Dysmetria of thought: clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunction on cognition and affect , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[11]  D L Na,et al.  Dissociation of sensory-attentional from motor-intentional neglect , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[12]  A. Cowey,et al.  Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  R. Tegnér,et al.  Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[15]  D. Neary Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology , 1990 .