The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate

Abstract In this review, we are pitting two theories against each other: the more accepted theory, the number sense theory, suggesting that a sense of number is innate and non-symbolic numerosity is being processed independently of continuous magnitudes (e.g., size, area, and density); and the newly emerging theory suggesting that (1) both numerosities and continuous magnitudes are processed holistically when comparing numerosities and (2) a sense of number might not be innate. In the first part of this review, we discuss the number sense theory. Against this background, we demonstrate how the natural correlation between numerosities and continuous magnitudes makes it nearly impossible to study non-symbolic numerosity processing in isolation from continuous magnitudes, and therefore, the results of behavioral and imaging studies with infants, adults, and animals can be explained, at least in part, by relying on continuous magnitudes. In the second part, we explain the sense of magnitude theory and review studies that directly demonstrate that continuous magnitudes are more automatic and basic than numerosities. Finally, we present outstanding questions. Our conclusion is that there is not enough convincing evidence to support the number sense theory anymore. Therefore, we encourage researchers not to assume that number sense is simply innate, but to put this hypothesis to the test and consider whether such an assumption is even testable in the light of the correlation of numerosity and continuous magnitudes.

[1]  Darko Odic Children's intuitive sense of number develops independently of their perception of area, density, length, and time. , 2018, Developmental science.

[2]  A. Henik,et al.  One tamed at a time: A new approach for controlling continuous magnitudes in numerical comparison tasks , 2017, Behavior research methods.

[3]  Yarden Gliksman,et al.  Size Perception and the Foundation of Numerical Processing , 2017 .

[4]  Joonkoo Park,et al.  Non-symbolic approximate arithmetic training improves math performance in preschoolers. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Probing the nature of deficits in the 'Approximate Number System' in children with persistent Developmental Dyscalculia. , 2016, Developmental science.

[6]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Grouping by proximity and the visual impression of approximate number in random dot arrays , 2016, Vision Research.

[7]  M. Inglis,et al.  Congruency effects in dot comparison tasks: convex hull is more important than dot area , 2016, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[8]  D. Burr,et al.  A shared numerical representation for action and perception , 2016, eLife.

[9]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Why numerical symbols count in the development of mathematical skills: evidence from brain and behavior , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[10]  Elisa Castaldi,et al.  Numerosity but not texture-density discrimination correlates with math ability in children. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[11]  Eriko Matsumoto,et al.  Numerosity underestimation in sets with illusory contours , 2016, Vision Research.

[12]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence, and outstanding questions. , 2016, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[13]  Ilyse Resnick,et al.  Developmental growth trajectories in understanding of fraction magnitude from fourth through sixth grade. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[14]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Individual Differences in Nonsymbolic Ratio Processing Predict Symbolic Math Performance , 2016, Psychological science.

[15]  Guido Marco Cicchini,et al.  Number As a Primary Perceptual Attribute: A Review , 2016, Perception.

[16]  Joonkoo Park,et al.  Rapid and Direct Encoding of Numerosity in the Visual Stream. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Asymmetric Processing of Numerical and Nonnumerical Magnitudes in the Brain: An fMRI Study , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Ratio dependence in small number discrimination is affected by the experimental procedure , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[19]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Numerosity processing is context driven even in the subitizing range: An fMRI study , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Sarah Clayton,et al.  Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: the effect of different visual controls on ANS measurement. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[21]  Dima Amso,et al.  The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Alexandra A. Cleland,et al.  The role of numerical and non-numerical cues in nonsymbolic number processing: Evidence from the line bisection task , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Modeling the approximate number system to quantify the contribution of visual stimulus features , 2015, Cognition.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: Symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.

[25]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders’ knowledge of fractions ☆ , 2015 .

[26]  Helen M. Ditz,et al.  Neurons selective to the number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza,et al.  Numerical and area comparison abilities in Down syndrome. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.

[28]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Inhibitory control may not explain the link between approximation and math abilities in kindergarteners from middle class families , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[29]  P. Viswanathan,et al.  Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons , 2015, Current Biology.

[30]  S Dehaene,et al.  Spatially invariant coding of numerical information in functionally defined subregions of human parietal cortex. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Set size and culture influence children's attention to number. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The effect of different methods to construct non-symbolic stimuli in numerosity estimation and comparison , 2015 .

[33]  Stella F. Lourenco On the Relation between Numerical and Non-Numerical Magnitudes: Evidence for a General Magnitude System , 2015 .

[34]  C. Gilmore,et al.  Inhibition in dot comparison tasks , 2015 .

[35]  David C. Burr,et al.  A generalized sense of number , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Neural adaptation to non-symbolic number and visual shape: An electrophysiological study , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[37]  Fatima Abbas,et al.  Fish Vision: Size Selectivity in the Zebrafish Retinotectal Pathway , 2014, Current Biology.

[38]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  The right temporo-parietal junction contributes to visual feature binding , 2014, NeuroImage.

[39]  Johann H. Bollmann,et al.  Classification of Object Size in Retinotectal Microcircuits , 2014, Current Biology.

[40]  Lisa K Fazio,et al.  Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[41]  Camilla Gilmore,et al.  Skills underlying mathematics: The role of executive function in the development of mathematics proficiency , 2014, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[42]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Comparing Performance in Discrete and Continuous Comparison Tasks , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The Neural Mechanism Underlying Ordinal Numerosity Processing , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  W. Gevers,et al.  Topographic representation of high-level cognition: numerosity or sensory processing? , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  David C. Burr,et al.  Separate Mechanisms for Perception of Numerosity and Density , 2014, Psychological science.

[46]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number trumps area for 7-month-old infants. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[47]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Transfer of Cognitive Training across Magnitude Dimensions Achieved with Concurrent Brain Stimulation of the Parietal Lobe , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  B. P. Klein,et al.  Topographic Representation of Numerosity in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2013, Science.

[49]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Open questions and a proposal: A critical review of the evidence on infant numerical abilities , 2013, Cognition.

[50]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[51]  Amy Devine,et al.  Visual stimulus parameters seriously compromise the measurement of approximate number system acuity and comparative effects between adults and children , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[52]  A. Ishiguchi,et al.  Effects of perceptual variables on numerosity comparison in 5–6-year-olds and adults , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[53]  Pooja Viswanathan,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a visual “sense of number” in primate parietal and prefrontal cortices , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Neil Marlow,et al.  Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.

[55]  L. Regolin,et al.  Numerical Abstraction in Young Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus) , 2013, PloS one.

[56]  Noam Siegelman,et al.  What Predicts Successful Literacy Acquisition in a Second Language? , 2013, Psychological science.

[57]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Magnitude processing in non-symbolic stimuli , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[58]  Tatiana Kornienko,et al.  Nature’s Measuring Tape: A Cognitive Basis for Adaptive Utility , 2013 .

[59]  D. Plaut,et al.  Distributed circuits, not circumscribed centers, mediate visual recognition , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The neural mechanisms underlying passive and active processing of numerosity , 2013, NeuroImage.

[61]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[62]  Willem Zuidema,et al.  Modeling in the language sciences , 2013 .

[63]  A. Henik,et al.  Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? , 2013, Developmental science.

[64]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Saccadic Compression of Symbolic Numerical Magnitude , 2012, PloS one.

[65]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Continuous visual properties explain neural responses to nonsymbolic number. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[66]  A. Oliva,et al.  A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[67]  Wolfgang Grodd,et al.  Comparison of quantities: core and format-dependent regions as revealed by fMRI. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The Role of Visual Information in Numerosity Estimation , 2012, PloS one.

[69]  Michael Andres,et al.  Contribution of the right intraparietal sulcus to numerosity and length processing: An fMRI-guided TMS study , 2012, Cortex.

[70]  Alain Content,et al.  Judgement of discrete and continuous quantity in adults: Number counts! , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[71]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies , 2012, PloS one.

[72]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Quantities, Amounts, and the Numerical Core System , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci.

[73]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[74]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Quantity judgments of auditory and visual stimuli by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[75]  L. Feigenson Predicting sights from sounds: 6-month-olds' intermodal numerical abilities. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[76]  S. Sugita,et al.  Quantity discrimination in jungle crows, Corvus macrorhynchos , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[77]  Christian Agrillo,et al.  Number versus continuous quantity in numerosity judgments by fish , 2011, Cognition.

[78]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[79]  E. Brannon,et al.  Attending to One of Many: When Infants are Surprisingly Poor at Discriminating an Item's Size , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[80]  Arlette Streri,et al.  Cues for Early Social Skills: Direct Gaze Modulates Newborns' Recognition of Talking Faces , 2011, PloS one.

[81]  Kerry E Jordan,et al.  Multisensory information boosts numerical matching abilities in young children. , 2011, Developmental science.

[82]  A. Young Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979) , 2011 .

[83]  Midori Tokita,et al.  How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[84]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Representations of visual proportions in the primate posterior parietal and prefrontal cortices , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[85]  P. Kuhl Brain Mechanisms in Early Language Acquisition , 2010, Neuron.

[86]  J. Morton Understanding genetic, neurophysiological, and experiential influences on the development of executive functioning: the need for developmental models. , 2010 .

[87]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[88]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.

[89]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[90]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Heritability of the Specific Cognitive Ability of Face Perception , 2010, Current Biology.

[91]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Common and segregated neural pathways for the processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude: An fMRI study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[92]  Christopher Y Olivola,et al.  Distributions of observed death tolls govern sensitivity to human fatalities , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[93]  Bertrand Thirion,et al.  Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns , 2009, Current Biology.

[94]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Tuning to non‐symbolic proportions in the human frontoparietal cortex , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[95]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The Neural Development of an Abstract Concept of Number , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  Susan Carey,et al.  Where Our Number Concepts Come From. , 2009, The journal of philosophy.

[97]  Ikuya Murakami,et al.  Perceived duration of visual motion increases with speed. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[98]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[99]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[100]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Contributions of primate prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices to length and numerosity representation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[101]  Sara Cordes,et al.  The relative salience of discrete and continuous quantity in young infants. , 2009, Developmental science.

[102]  Marco Dadda,et al.  Use of Number by Fish , 2009, PloS one.

[103]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Adult brains don't fully overcome biases that lead to incorrect performance during cognitive development: an fMRI study in young adults completing a Piaget-like task. , 2009, Developmental science.

[104]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Beyond the number domain , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[105]  J. Taube,et al.  Where am I and how will I get there from here? A role for posterior parietal cortex in the integration of spatial information and route planning , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[106]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia: heterogeneity might not mean different mechanisms , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[107]  J. Tautz,et al.  Number-Based Visual Generalisation in the Honeybee , 2009, PloS one.

[108]  A. Adler,et al.  Configural Processing , 2009, Encyclopedia of Biometrics.

[109]  P. E. Pisa,et al.  Quantity discrimination in felines: a preliminary investigation of the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) , 2009, Journal of Ethology.

[110]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Matching Categorical Object Representations in Inferior Temporal Cortex of Man and Monkey , 2008, Neuron.

[111]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  From magnitude to natural numbers: A developmental neurocognitive perspective , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[112]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[113]  F. Durgin Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited , 2008, Current Biology.

[114]  Sumarga H. Suanda,et al.  Intersensory redundancy accelerates preverbal numerical competence , 2008, Cognition.

[115]  S. Wiebe,et al.  Short-Term Memory, Working Memory, and Executive Functioning in Preschoolers: Longitudinal Predictors of Mathematical Achievement at Age 7 Years , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[116]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[117]  Marco Dadda,et al.  Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[118]  Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[119]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Anchors aweigh: A demonstration of cross-modality anchoring and magnitude priming , 2008, Cognition.

[120]  D. Burr,et al.  A Visual Sense of Number , 2007, Current Biology.

[121]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Neuronal population coding of continuous and discrete quantity in the primate posterior parietal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[122]  Sheng He,et al.  Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[123]  Maxwel Roberts,et al.  Integrating the Mind , 2007 .

[124]  C. Blair,et al.  Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. , 2007, Child development.

[125]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) enumerate large and small sequentially presented sets of items using analog numerical representations. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[126]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Sometimes area counts more than number , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[127]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[128]  J. Cantlon,et al.  Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.

[129]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[130]  S. Gathercole,et al.  Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[131]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[132]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The multisensory representation of number in infancy. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[133]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Do we need a number sense ? , 2006 .

[134]  Hilary Barth,et al.  Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[135]  Thomas R. Knösche,et al.  Who Comes First? The Role of the Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[136]  Stefan Golaszewski,et al.  Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[137]  A. Nieder Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[138]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[139]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[140]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[141]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[142]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[143]  T. Senn,et al.  The Contribution of Executive Functions to Emergent Mathematic Skills in Preschool Children , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[144]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[145]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[146]  R. Cheloha,et al.  The of a Development , 2004 .

[147]  R. Diehl,et al.  Speech Perception , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[148]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[149]  Lisa Feigenson,et al.  Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants' manual search , 2003 .

[150]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[151]  Fei Xu,et al.  Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.

[152]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[153]  Fei Xu Numerosity discrimination in infants : Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003 .

[154]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: an investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[155]  John C Gore,et al.  The role of the parietal cortex in visual feature binding , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[156]  P. Anderson Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) During Childhood , 2002, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[157]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[158]  Duane M. Rumbaugh,et al.  "Constructive" enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) on a computerized task , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[159]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[160]  G. Scerif,et al.  Executive Functioning as a Predictor of Children's Mathematics Ability: Inhibition, Switching, and Working Memory , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[161]  Susan Carey,et al.  Infants' knowledge of objects: beyond object files and object tracking , 2001, Cognition.

[162]  D. Rakison,et al.  Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[163]  Susan Carey,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis , 2001 .

[164]  H. Freund,et al.  The role of the inferior parietal cortex in linking the tactile perception and manual construction of object shapes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[165]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[166]  L Girelli,et al.  The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[167]  R. Campbell,et al.  Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging of crossmodal binding in the human heteromodal cortex , 2000, Current Biology.

[168]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[169]  Gert Westermann,et al.  A constructivist dual-representation model of verb inflection , 2000 .

[170]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[171]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  An Area within Human Ventral Cortex Sensitive to “Building” Stimuli Evidence and Implications , 1998, Neuron.

[172]  Shigeru Watanabe Discrimination of “Four” and “Two” by Pigeons , 1998 .

[173]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[174]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[175]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.

[176]  S. Carey,et al.  Infants’ Metaphysics: The Case of Numerical Identity , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[177]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Perception of object unity in 2-month-old infants. , 1995 .

[178]  Scott W. Brown Time, change, and motion: The effects of stimulus movement on temporal perception , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[179]  C. Packer,et al.  Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[180]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[181]  T. Tuulmets,et al.  Occupancy model of perceived numerosity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[182]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[183]  R. Shalev,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia , 2004, Journal of child neurology.

[184]  N Ginsburg,et al.  Perceived Numerosity as a Function of Item Size , 1988, Perceptual and motor skills.

[185]  D. Burr,et al.  Visual processing of motion , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[186]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Use of numbers by a chimpanzee , 1985, Nature.

[187]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Neonatal perception of spatial relationships , 1985 .

[188]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[189]  Daniel P. Keating,et al.  Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. , 1983, Child development.

[190]  L. E. Krueger,et al.  Single judgments of numerosity , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[191]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[192]  M S Banks,et al.  The development of visual accommodation during early infancy. , 1980, Child development.

[193]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[194]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[195]  Velma Dobson,et al.  Visual acuity in human infants: A review and comparison of behavioral and electrophysiological studies , 1978, Vision Research.

[196]  D. Hubel,et al.  Ferrier lecture - Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1977, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[197]  C. C. Goren,et al.  Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. , 1975, Pediatrics.

[198]  Peter D. Eimas,et al.  Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-l] distinction by young infants , 1975 .

[199]  C. Frith,et al.  The solitaire illusion: An illusion of numerosity , 1972 .

[200]  L. Calhoun Number Conservation in Very Young Children: The Effect of Age and Mode of Responding. , 1971 .

[201]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[202]  W. Pitts,et al.  What the Frog's Eye Tells the Frog's Brain , 1959, Proceedings of the IRE.

[203]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Number , 1953 .

[204]  E. L. Kaufman,et al.  The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.

[205]  J. B. S. HALDANE,et al.  Number: the Language of Science , 1941, Nature.