The myth of the visual word form area

Recent functional imaging studies have referred to a posterior region of the left midfusiform gyrus as the "visual word form area" (VWFA). We review the evidence for this claim and argue that neither the neuropsychological nor neuroimaging data are consistent with a cortical region specialized for visual word form representations. Specifically, there are no reported cases of pure alexia who have deficits limited to visual word form processing and damage limited to the left midfusiform. In addition, we present functional imaging data to demonstrate that the so-called VWFA is activated by normal subjects during tasks that do not engage visual word form processing such as naming colors, naming pictures, reading Braille, repeating auditory words, and making manual action responses to pictures of meaningless objects. If the midfusiform region has a single function that underlies all these tasks, then it does not correspond to visual word form processing. On the other hand, if the region participates in several functions as defined by its interactions with other cortical areas, then identifying the neural system sustaining visual word form representations requires identification of the set of regions involved. We conclude that there is no evidence that visual word form representations are subtended by a single patch of neuronal cortex and it is misleading to label the left midfusiform region as the visual word form area.

[1]  N. Geschwind Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. II. , 1965, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  A P Leff,et al.  The functional anatomy of single-word reading in patients with hemianopic and pure alexia. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  C. Price,et al.  The Constraints Functional Neuroimaging Places on Classical Models of Auditory Word Processing , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  A. Damasio,et al.  Cortical systems for retrieval of concrete knowledge: The convergence zone framework , 1994 .

[6]  K. Patterson,et al.  Speak and spell: Dissociations and word-class effects. , 1987 .

[7]  Michael P. Alexander,et al.  Pictures, images, and pure alexia: A case study , 1984 .

[8]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Differential activation of right and left posterior sylvian regions by semantic and phonological tasks: a positron-emission tomography study in normal human subjects , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[9]  James R. Booth,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Intra- and Cross-Modal Lexical Tasks , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Picture Naming , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Alex Martin,et al.  Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Coltheart,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of language , 1987 .

[15]  K. M. Heilman,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychological Analysis and Neuroanatomic Correlates in a Case of Acute Anomia , 1997, Brain and Language.

[16]  A. M. Fasanaro,et al.  [The alexias]. , 1985, Rivista di neurologia.

[17]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Function of the left planum temporale in auditory and linguistic processing , 1996, NeuroImage.

[18]  Anne L. Foundas,et al.  Anomia: Case studies with lesion localization , 1998 .

[19]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  V Bettinardi,et al.  The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Lippincott Williams Wilkins Basal temporal language area demonstrated by electrical stimulation , 1986 .

[22]  D. Beversdorf,et al.  Pure alexia: clinical-pathologic evidence for a lateralized visual language association cortex , 2000 .

[23]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The neural substrates of action retrieval: An examination of semantic and visual routes to action , 2002 .

[24]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[25]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  J. Dejerine,et al.  Contribution a l'etude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale , 2000 .

[27]  C. Price,et al.  Three Distinct Ventral Occipitotemporal Regions for Reading and Object Naming , 1999, NeuroImage.

[28]  J. Binder,et al.  The topography of callosal reading pathways. A case-control analysis. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Modality independence of word comprehension , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[30]  Philipp Slusallek,et al.  Introduction to real-time ray tracing , 2005, SIGGRAPH Courses.

[31]  J. Marshall,et al.  Patterns of paralexia: a psycholinguistic approach. , 1995, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[32]  E. Warrington Word-form dyslexia , 1995 .

[33]  S. Scott,et al.  Noun imageability and the temporal lobes , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The neural regions sustaining object recognition and naming , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Hierarchical Organization of the Human Auditory Cortex Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Alex Martin,et al.  Experience-dependent modulation of category-related cortical activity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  M. Farah,et al.  A neural basis for category and modality specificity of semantic knowledge , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  E. Renzi,et al.  The pattern of neuropsychological impairment associated with left posterior cerebral artery infarcts. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures , 1996, Nature.

[40]  A. Damasio,et al.  The anatomic basis of pure alexia , 1983, Neurology.

[41]  Hannu J. Aronen,et al.  Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2000, Neurobiology of Aging.

[42]  M. Kurachi,et al.  Recovery from Alexia Without Agraphia: Report of an Autopsy , 1979, Cortex.

[43]  M. Farah,et al.  A functional MRI study of mental image generation , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[47]  R P Lesser,et al.  Characterization of the basal temporal language area in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy , 1990, Neurology.

[48]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway , 1998, Nature.

[50]  C. J. Price,et al.  Cortical localisation of the visual and auditory word form areas: A reconsideration of the evidence , 2003, Brain and Language.

[51]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading , 1995 .

[52]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[53]  S. Petersen,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of word reading. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[54]  U. Frith,et al.  Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  D. Schacter,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[56]  M. Denis,et al.  Cortical anatomy of mental imagery of concrete nouns based on their dictionary definition , 1998, Neuroreport.

[57]  Thad A Polk,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for an abstract, not perceptual, word-form area. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Visual complexity in letter-by-letter reading: Pure alexia is not pure , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  H A Whitaker,et al.  The bilingual brain. , 1978, Archives of neurology.

[60]  R P Lesser,et al.  Basal temporal language area demonstrated by electrical stimulation , 1986, Neurology.

[61]  B. Rosen,et al.  Auditory and visual word processing studied with fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[62]  Richard Durbin,et al.  A dimension reduction framework for understanding cortical maps , 1990, Nature.

[63]  Emmanuel Mellet,et al.  Picture naming without Broca's and Wernicke's area , 2000, Neuroreport.

[64]  J. Haxby,et al.  Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.

[66]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  Towards a network theory of cognition , 2000, Neural Networks.

[67]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[68]  Klaas E. Stephan,et al.  The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.