Upper limb asymmetries in the perception of proprioceptively determined dynamic position sense.

Recent studies of position-related proprioceptive sense have provided evidence of a nonpreferred left arm advantage in right-handed individuals. The present study sought to determine whether similar asymmetries might exist in "dynamic position" sense. Thirteen healthy, right-handed adults were blindfolded and seated with arms placed on instrumented manipulanda. In Part 1, subjects performed dynamic position matching of 3 target elbow amplitudes determined with the preferred or nonpreferred arm, and then matched during movement of the same or opposite elbow. In Part 2, a similar paradigm was used, but with varying target determination speeds to account for the so called "tau effect." Overall, it was found that errors were smaller when the matching phase involved the nonpreferred arm, especially for larger target amplitudes. This asymmetry was independent of the tau effect and likely reflects specialization of the right hemisphere/left arm for proprioceptive feedback processing that is either position- or dynamic position-related.

[1]  Daniel J. Goble,et al.  Dynamic proprioceptive target matching behavior in the upper limb: Effects of speed, task difficulty and arm/hemisphere asymmetries , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dominance of the right hemisphere and role of area 2 in human kinesthesia. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Eiichi Naito,et al.  Human limb‐specific and non‐limb‐specific brain representations during kinesthetic illusory movements of the upper and lower extremities , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  L. Arendt-Nielsen,et al.  Effects of NGF-induced muscle sensitization on proprioception and nociception , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  P J Cordo,et al.  Kinesthetic control of a multijoint movement sequence. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  H. Helson The tau effect—an example of psychological relativity. , 1930 .

[7]  D. Goble,et al.  Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  J. Velay,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry and interhemispheric transferin reaching programming , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Proprioceptive Illusions Induced by Muscle Vibration: Contribution by Muscle Spindles to Perception? , 1972, Science.

[11]  V. Nougier,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Reaching Movement Control. I: Study of Right-Handers , 2001, Cortex.

[12]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of discrete movement sequences: mechanism and generality. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[13]  B. Milner,et al.  Recall of self-generated arm movements by patients with unilateral cortical excisions , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  D. Goble,et al.  Reply to Dr. Derakhshan , 2008 .

[17]  J. Hellige Cerebral hemisphere asymmetry: Method, theory, and application , 1983 .

[18]  E A Roy,et al.  Absolute error. , 1973, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  Eric A. Roy,et al.  Handedness Effects in Kinesthetic Spatial Location Judgements , 1978, Cortex.

[20]  Charles J. Wysocki,et al.  Hand preference and age in the United States , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  M. Hollins,et al.  Perception of the length of voluntary movements. , 1988, Somatosensory research.

[22]  D. Goble,et al.  Task-dependent asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback for goal-directed movement , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Daniel J Goble,et al.  Deficits in the ability to use proprioceptive feedback in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy , 2009, International journal of rehabilitation research. Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung. Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation.

[24]  V Nougier,et al.  Control of goal-directed movements: the contribution of orienting of visual attention and motor preparation. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Neural mechanisms underlying reaching for remembered targets cued kinesthetically or visually in left or right hemispace , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[26]  L. Riolo-Quinn Relationship of hand preference to accuracy on a thumb-positioning task. , 1991, Perceptual and motor skills.

[27]  Daniel J. Goble,et al.  Proprioceptive target matching asymmetries in left-handed individuals , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  D. Burke,et al.  The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration of non‐contracting muscles. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  D. Burke,et al.  The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration during isometric contraction. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  S. Chieffi,et al.  Movement velocity effects on kinaesthetic localisation of spatial positions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  U. Windhorst,et al.  Correlation of the dynamic behaviour of deefferented primary muscle spindle endings with their static behaviour , 2004, Pflügers Archiv.

[32]  K Imanaka,et al.  Interference Between Location and Distance Information in Motor Short-Term Memory: The Respective Roles of Direct Kinesthetic Signals and Abstract Codes. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  Daniel J Goble,et al.  Development of upper limb proprioceptive accuracy in children and adolescents. , 2005, Human movement science.

[34]  N. Sharma,et al.  Left-Arm Dominance in Active Positioning , 1989, Perceptual and motor skills.

[35]  A. Colley Spatial Location Judgements by Right and Left-Handers , 1984, Cortex.

[36]  D. Goble,et al.  The biological and behavioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensorimotor performance , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Cognitive Strategies and Short-term Memory for Movement Distance and Location , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[38]  D. Goble,et al.  Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Manual reaction time asymmetries in human subjects: the role of movement planning and attention , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[41]  B. Milner,et al.  Recall of the end-position of examiner-defined arm movements by patients with frontal- or temporal-lobe lesions , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  M. Annett Left, right, hand and brain : the right shift theory , 1985 .

[43]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Limitations in interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  R. Bianconi,et al.  The response to vibration of the end organs of mammalian muscle spindles. , 1963, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Gabriel Leonard,et al.  Contribution of the right frontal lobe to the encoding and recall of kinesthetic distance information , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  H. Helson THE TAU EFFECT--AN EXAMPLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RELATIVITY. , 1931, Science.

[48]  V. Gurfinkel,et al.  Proprioceptive consequences of tendon vibration during movement. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.