Perceptual organization of behavior: a hierarchical control model of coordinated action.

The behavior of individual subjects is compared with a hierarchical control system model of behavioral organization. Subjects varied the position of two control handles simultaneously to keep the distance constant between two pairs of lines. Three variations on this basic experiment that illustrate some fundamental properties of coordinated action are shown: first, how independent actions, compensating for unpredictable and undetectable disturbances, can produce a single behavioral result; second, how the ability to produce a particular result is maintained when the connection between action and result is changed; and third, how two independent outputs can appear to be related as coordinative structures when one output disturbs a result being controlled by the other. The correlation between the behavior of subjects and model in all experiments is typically on the order of .99. A detailed examination of the operation of the model shows that actions are structured by perception, not by central commands or equations of constraint.

[1]  Elliot Saltzman,et al.  Skilled actions: a task-dynamic approach. , 1987, Psychological review.

[2]  J. Kelso Human Motor Behavior: An Introduction , 1982 .

[3]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Skill Acquisition: An Event Approach with Special Reference to Searching for the Optimum of a Function of Several Variables , 1978 .

[4]  J. Adams,et al.  A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Effect of load disturbances during centrally initiated movements. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  R. Marken,et al.  Intentional and Accidental Behavior: A Control Theory Analysis , 1982 .

[7]  W. T. Powers Quantitative Analysis of Purposive Systems: Some Spadework at the Foundations of Scientific Psychology , 1978 .

[8]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[9]  J. Kelso Motor control mechanisms underlying human movement reproduction. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  R. Schmidt Control Processes In Motor Skills , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[11]  W. T. Powers Behavior, the control of perception , 1973 .

[12]  E. Brunswik,et al.  The Conceptual Framework of Psychology , 1954 .

[13]  M. Gazzaniga Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience , 1984, Springer US.

[14]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Human information processing : tutorials in performance and cognition , 1975 .

[15]  John R. Searle,et al.  The Intentionality of Intention and Action , 1980, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Tutorials in learning and memory : essays in honor of Gordon Bower , 1984 .

[17]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .

[18]  J. Kelso,et al.  A Dynamical Basis for Action Systems , 1984 .

[19]  M. T. Turvey,et al.  Issues in the theory of action : Degree of freedom, coordinative structures and coalitions , 1978 .

[20]  D Goodman,et al.  On the nature of human interlimb coordination. , 1979, Science.

[21]  D. Norman Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .

[22]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[23]  J. Requin Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .

[24]  J. Kelso,et al.  Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: evidence for coordinative structures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R Marken The Cause of Control Movements in a Tracking Task , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[27]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Attention and human performance , 1973 .

[28]  Thomas A. McMahon,et al.  Muscles, Reflexes, and Locomotion , 1984 .

[29]  G. Stelmach Information processing in motor control and learning , 1978 .

[30]  R. W. Pew,et al.  Human perceptual-motor performance , 1974 .