Detection of geometric, but not topological, spatial transformations in 6- to 12-month-old infants in a visual exploration paradigm.

Several theories of spatial orientation propose that the geometry of an environment plays a privileged role in reorientation, relative to relations between individual landmarks. Infants (N = 90) in three age groups (6, 8 1/2, and 12 months) experienced three conditions: topological, geometric, and control. A round room contained four distinctive objects in a rectangular arrangement on the inside periphery. Infants were familiarized to the array prior to a 2-min test period. In the topological condition, two objects were switched. In the geometric condition, the objects were moved to form an irregular quadrilateral. In the control condition, the array remained unchanged. Infants of 8 1/2 months and over visually explored significantly more in the geometric condition only. An initial study with adults found greater visual exploration in both geometric and topological conditions. These results are discussed in the context of current theories of spatial orientation.

[1]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[2]  A. Fenton,et al.  Place navigation in the Morris water maze under minimum and redundant extra-maze cue conditions. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[3]  R. Muller,et al.  Further study of the control of place cell firing by intra‐apparatus objects , 1999, Hippocampus.

[4]  E. Spelke,et al.  Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space , 2001, Cognition.

[5]  E. Save,et al.  Drawing parallels between the behavioural and neural properties of navigation , 2003 .

[6]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  LANDMARK USE BY NAVIGATING RATS (RATTUS NORVEGICUS) : CONTRASTING GEOMETRIC AND FEATURAL INFORMATION , 1998 .

[7]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  The development of the temporal and nasal visual fields during infancy , 1992, Vision Research.

[8]  J. Taube,et al.  Correlation between head direction cell activity and spatial behavior on a radial arm maze. , 1997 .

[9]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[10]  Adina R. Lew,et al.  Indirect landmark use at 6 months of age in a spatial orientation task. , 2004 .

[11]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[12]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Spatial coding in the hippocampal formation: input, information type, plasticity and behaviour. , 2003 .

[13]  E. Rolls,et al.  View‐responsive neurons in the primate hippocampal complex , 1995, Hippocampus.

[14]  G. M. Martin,et al.  Spatial disorientation blocks reliable goal location on a plus maze but does not prevent goal location in the Morris maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[15]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Travel Broadens the Mind. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[16]  B. Poucet Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. , 1993, Psychological review.

[17]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Background, But Not Foreground, Spatial Cues Are Taken as References for Head Direction Responses by Rat Anterodorsal Thalamus Neurons , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Development of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for early learning in an eye-tracking paradigm , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[21]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  R. Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  G. Bronson,et al.  Infant differences in rate of visual encoding. , 1991, Child development.

[24]  J. G. Bremner,et al.  The development of relational landmark use in six- to twelve-month-old infants in a spatial orientation task. , 2000, Child development.

[25]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[29]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[30]  M. Pondel,et al.  During stepwise cue deletion, rat place behaviors correlate with place unit responses , 1985, Brain Research.

[31]  E. Spelke,et al.  Updating egocentric representations in human navigation , 2000, Cognition.

[32]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  A study of exploratory behavior as an index of spatial knowledge in hamsters , 1986 .

[33]  J. Taube,et al.  Effects of repeated disorientation on the acquisition of spatial tasks in rats: dissociation between the appetitive radial arm maze and aversive water maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  L. Acredolo Development of Spatial Orientation in Infancy. , 1978 .

[35]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[36]  J. Gavin Bremner,et al.  Use of cue configuration geometry for spatial orientation in human infants (Homo sapiens). , 2001 .

[37]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior. , 1980 .

[38]  R. Muller,et al.  Failure of Centrally Placed Objects to Control the Firing Fields of Hippocampal Place Cells , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Kathryn J. Jeffery,et al.  The neurobiology of spatial behaviour , 2003 .