Hard to catch: Experimental evidence supports evasive mimicry

Most research on aposematism has focused on chemically defended prey, but signalling difficulty of capture remains poorly explored. Similarly to classical Batesian and Müllerian mimicry related to distastefulness, such “evasive aposematism” may also lead to convergence in warning colours, known as evasive mimicry. A prime candidate group for evasive mimicry are Adelpha butterflies, which are agile insects and show remarkable colour pattern convergence. We tested the ability of naïve blue tits to learn to avoid and generalise Adelpha wing patterns associated with difficulty of capture, and compared their response to that of birds that learned to associate the same wing patterns with distastefulness. Birds learned to avoid all wing patterns tested, but learning was faster with evasive prey compared with distasteful prey. Birds generalised their learned avoidance from evasive models to imperfect mimics if the mimic shared colours with the model. Despite imperfect mimics gaining protection from bird’s generalisation, perfect mimics always had the best fitness, supporting selection for accurate mimicry. Faster avoidance learning and broader generalisation of evasive prey suggest that being hard to catch may deter predators at least as effectively as distastefulness. Our results provide empirical evidence for a potentially widespread alternative scenario, evasive mimicry, for the evolution of similar aposematic colour patterns.

[1]  C. Rowe,et al.  Learning about aposematic prey , 2016 .

[2]  T. Guerra Evasive mimicry: Too beetle, or not too beetle? , 2019, Ecology.

[3]  J. Mappes,et al.  Visual illusions in predator–prey interactions: birds find moving patterned prey harder to catch , 2015, Animal Cognition.

[4]  Susan D. Finkbeiner,et al.  Warning signals are seductive: Relative contributions of color and pattern to predator avoidance and mate attraction in Heliconius butterflies , 2014, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[5]  R. B. Srygley Locomotor Mimicry in Butterflies? The Associations of Positions of Centres of Mass among Groups of Mimetic, Unprofitable Prey , 1994 .

[6]  J. Mappes,et al.  Imperfect Batesian mimicry—the effects of the frequency and the distastefulness of the model , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  K. Willmott Cladistic analysis of the Neotropical butterfly genus Adelpha (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae), with comments on the subtribal classification of Limenitidini , 2003 .

[8]  R. B. Srygley,et al.  Incorporating Motion into Investigations of mimicry , 1999, Evolutionary Ecology.

[9]  L. Coppinger,et al.  Ecological Chemistry and the Palatability Spectrum , 1968, Science.

[10]  Pre-adaptation and speed mimicry among Namib Desert scarabaeids with orange elytra , 1979 .

[11]  S. Weller The Butterflies of Costa Rica and Their Natural History , 1998 .

[12]  C. Pinheiro Palatablility and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (Tyrannus melancholicus, Tyrannidae) , 1996 .

[13]  A. Aiello Adelpha (Nymphalidae): Deception on the wing , 1984 .

[14]  C. Rowe,et al.  Predators' Toxin Burdens Influence Their Strategic Decisions to Eat Toxic Prey , 2007, Current Biology.

[15]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Cognitive dimensions of predator responses to imperfect mimicry. , 2007, PLoS biology.

[16]  Susan D. Finkbeiner,et al.  Complex dynamics underlie the evolution of imperfect wing pattern convergence in butterflies , 2017, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[17]  I. Sneddon,et al.  The effect of distastefulness of the model on the predation of artificial batesian mimics , 1977, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  V. Jarošı́k,et al.  Avoidance of aposematic prey in European tits (Paridae): learned or innate? , 2007 .

[19]  K. Summers,et al.  Molecular phylogenetic evidence for a mimetic radiation in Peruvian poison frogs supports a Müllerian mimicry hypothesis , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  E. Poulton The colours of animals, their meaning and use, especially considered in the case of insects; by Edward Bagnall Poulton ... With chromolithographic frontispiece, and sixty-six figures in text. , .

[21]  E. Terhune Components of a Visual Stimulus Used by Scrub Jays to Discriminate a Batesian Model , 1977, The American Naturalist.

[22]  G. Ruxton,et al.  Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry , 2004 .

[23]  J. Mallet,et al.  Individual selection, kin selection, and the shifting balance in the evolution of warning colours: the evidence from butterflies , 1987 .

[24]  R. I. Hill,et al.  Frequency dependence shapes the adaptive landscape of imperfect Batesian mimicry , 2018, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  D. O. Gibson The role of escape in mimicry and polymorphism: I. The response of captive birds to artificial prey , 1980 .

[26]  J. Mappes,et al.  Colour alone matters: no predator generalization among morphs of an aposematic moth , 2018, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  M. Forister,et al.  North American velvet ants form one of the world’s largest known Müllerian mimicry complexes , 2015, Current Biology.

[28]  C. H. Lindroth Disappearance as a Protective Factor , 1971 .

[29]  J. Endler,et al.  The complex business of survival by aposematism. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[30]  K. Gerow,et al.  Experimental Evidence for Aposematism in the Dendrobatid Poison Frog Oophaga pumilio , 2007, Copeia.

[31]  C. H. Oh,et al.  Some comments on , 1998 .

[32]  G. Ruxton,et al.  Evasive mimicry: when (if ever) could mimicry based on difficulty of capture evolve? , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Hannah M. Rowland,et al.  Social information use about novel aposematic prey is not influenced by a predator's previous experience with toxins , 2019, Functional Ecology.

[34]  J. Endler,et al.  Predator Mixes and the Conspicuousness of Aposematic Signals , 2004, The American Naturalist.

[35]  M. Elias,et al.  Maintaining mimicry diversity: optimal warning colour patterns differ among microhabitats in Amazonian clearwing butterflies , 2017, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  C. J. Duncan,et al.  Sensory discrimination and its role in the evolution of Batesian mimicry. , 1965, Behaviour.

[37]  J. Mappes,et al.  Inter-species variation in unpalatability does not explain polymorphism in a mimetic species , 2016, Evolutionary Ecology.

[38]  L. Chittka,et al.  Cognitive Dimensions of Predator Responses to Imperfect Mimicry , 2007, PLoS Biology.

[39]  Some Possible Cases of Escape Mimicry in Neotropical Butterflies , 2014, Neotropical Entomology.

[40]  J. A. Allen,et al.  A simulation of evasive mimicry in the wild , 1991 .

[41]  O Hasson,et al.  Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator. , 1991, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[42]  O. Leimar,et al.  Stimulus Salience as an Explanation for Imperfect Mimicry , 2014, Current Biology.

[43]  G. Gamberale-Stille,et al.  Domestic chicks primarily attend to colour, not pattern, when learning an aposematic coloration , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[44]  R. I. Hill,et al.  Rapid diversification associated with ecological specialization in Neotropical Adelpha butterflies , 2015, Molecular ecology.

[45]  E. Poulton Adaptive Coloration in Animals , 1940, Nature.

[46]  R. I. Hill,et al.  Adult Feeding as a Potential Mechanism for Unprofitability in Neotropical Adelpha (Limenitidini, Limenitidinae, Nymphalidae) , 2019, The Journal of the Lepidopterists' Society.

[47]  David B. Ritland,et al.  Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies (Limenitis archippus) from four south-eastern United States populations , 1995, Oecologia.

[48]  Martin Stevens,et al.  The effect of predator appetite, prey warning coloration and luminance on predator foraging decisions , 2010 .

[49]  Hannah M. Rowland,et al.  Prey community structure affects how predators select for Müllerian mimicry , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  J. Mallet,et al.  Mimicry and warning colour at the boundary between races and species , 1998 .

[51]  J. Bond,et al.  A Müllerian mimicry ring in Appalachian millipedes , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  C. Pinheiro,et al.  The responses of wild jacamars (Galbula ruficauda, Galbulidae) to aposematic, aposematic and cryptic, and cryptic butterflies in central Brazil , 2019, Ecological Entomology.

[53]  K. S. Brown Mimicry, aposematism and crypsis in enotropical Lepidoptera: the importance of dual signals , 1988 .

[54]  D. O. Gibson Batesian mimicry without distastefulness? , 1974, Nature.

[55]  Does Müllerian Mimicry Work in Nature? Experiments with Butterflies and Birds (Tyrannidae)1 , 2003 .

[56]  E. Poulton The colours of animals: Their meaning and use: Especially considered in the case of insects , 2009 .

[57]  Jane Van Zandt Brower,et al.  EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF MIMICRY IN SOME NORTH AMERICAN BUTTERFLIES: PART I. THE MONARCH, DANAUS PLEXIPPUS, AND VICEROY, LIMENITIS ARCHIPPUS ARCHIPPUS , 1958 .

[58]  R. B. Srygley,et al.  Predation and the Flight, Morphology, and Temperature of Neotropical Rain-Forest Butterflies , 1990, The American Naturalist.

[59]  K. Willmott The genus Adelpha: its systematics, biology and biogeography (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae: Limenitidini). , 2003 .

[60]  R. B. Srygley,et al.  CORRELATIONS OF THE POSITION OF CENTER OF BODY MASS WITH BUTTERFLY ESCAPE TACTICS , 1993 .

[61]  G. Beccaloni Ecology, natural history and behaviour of Ithomiine butterflies and their mimics in Ecuador (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae: Ithomiinae). , 1997 .

[62]  M. Théry,et al.  Crossing fitness valleys: empirical estimation of a fitness landscape associated with polymorphic mimicry , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.