Perception of object motion in three-dimensional space induced by cast shadows

Cast shadows can be salient depth cues in three-dimensional (3D) vision. Using a motion illusion in which a ball is perceived to roll in depth on the bottom or to flow in the front plane depending on the slope of the trajectory of its cast shadow, we investigated cortical mechanisms underlying 3D vision based on cast shadows using fMRI techniques. When modified versions of the original illusion, in which the slope of the shadow trajectory (shadow slope) was changed in 5 steps from the same one as the ball trajectory to the horizontal, were presented to participants, their perceived ball trajectory shifted gradually from rolling on the bottom to floating in the front plane as the change of the shadow slope. This observation suggests that the perception of the ball trajectory in this illusion is strongly affected by the motion of the cast shadow. In the fMRI study, cortical activity during observation of the movies of the illusion was investigated. We found that the bilateral posterior-occipital sulcus (POS) and right ventral precuneus showed activation related to the perception of the ball trajectory induced by the cast shadows in the illusion. Of these areas, it was suggested that the right POS may be involved in the inferring of the ball trajectory by the given spatial relation between the ball and the shadow. Our present results suggest that the posterior portion of the medial parietal cortex may be involved in 3D vision by cast shadows.

[1]  François Michel,et al.  Seeing without the Occipito-Parietal Cortex: Simultagnosia as a Shrinkage of the Attentional Visual Field , 2004, Behavioural neurology.

[2]  Ernst Martin,et al.  Dorsal stream development in motion and structure-from-motion perception , 2008, NeuroImage.

[3]  Sabrina Pitzalis,et al.  The cortical visual area V6 in macaque and human brains , 2009, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[4]  Akihiro Yagi,et al.  Perception of motion trajectory of object from the moving cast shadow in infants , 2006, Vision Research.

[5]  Ronald R. Peeters,et al.  Mapping multiple visual areas in the human brain with a short fMRI sequence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[6]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Localization of human intraparietal areas AIP, CIP, and LIP using surface orientation and saccadic eye movement tasks , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[7]  Guy Marchal,et al.  Human Cortical Regions Involved in Extracting Depth from Motion , 1999, Neuron.

[8]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The Extraction of 3D Shape from Texture and Shading in the Human Brain , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  3D surface perception from motion involves a temporal–parietal network , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  W. Becker,et al.  Deriving angular displacement from optic flow: a fMRI study , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[13]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Combination of hand and gaze signals during reaching: activity in parietal area 7 m of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  M. Taira,et al.  Cortical Areas Related to Attention to 3D Surface Structures Based on Shading: An fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[15]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Sensitivity to optic flow in human cortical areas MT and MST , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[17]  S. Shafer Shadows and Silhouettes in Computer Vision , 1985 .

[18]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Distinguishing subregions of the human MT+ complex using visual fields and pursuit eye movements. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  A Berthoz,et al.  Visual perception of motion and 3-D structure from motion: an fMRI study. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  D Kersten,et al.  Moving Cast Shadows Induce Apparent Motion in Depth , 1997, Perception.

[21]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Impossible Shadows and the Shadow Correspondence Problem , 2004, Perception.

[22]  P. P. Battaglini,et al.  Parietal neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordinates , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  S. Zeki,et al.  The cerebral activity related to the visual perception of forward motion in depth. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Natural vision reveals regional specialization to local motion and to contrast-invariant, global flow in the human brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  D. Knill,et al.  The perception of cast shadows , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  S. Palmer Vision Science : Photons to Phenomenology , 1999 .

[27]  M. Ptito,et al.  Cortical Representation of Inward and Outward Radial Motion in Man , 2001, NeuroImage.

[28]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Shape from shadows. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Tomoka Naganuma,et al.  Information processing of geometrical features of a surface based on binocular disparity cues: an fMRI study , 2005, Neuroscience Research.

[30]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Functional imaging of brain areas involved in the processing of coherent and incoherent wide field-of-view visual motion , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.