Proprioceptive loss and the perception, control and learning of arm movements in humans: evidence from sensory neuronopathy

It is uncertain how vision and proprioception contribute to adaptation of voluntary arm movements. In normal participants, adaptation to imposed forces is possible with or without vision, suggesting that proprioception is sufficient; in participants with proprioceptive loss (PL), adaptation is possible with visual feedback, suggesting that proprioception is unnecessary. In experiment 1 adaptation to, and retention of, perturbing forces were evaluated in three chronically deafferented participants. They made rapid reaching movements to move a cursor toward a visual target, and a planar robot arm applied orthogonal velocity-dependent forces. Trial-by-trial error correction was observed in all participants. Such adaptation has been characterized with a dual-rate model: a fast process that learns quickly, but retains poorly and a slow process that learns slowly and retains well. Experiment 2 showed that the PL participants had large individual differences in learning and retention rates compared to normal controls. Experiment 3 tested participants’ perception of applied forces. With visual feedback, the PL participants could report the perturbation’s direction as well as controls; without visual feedback, thresholds were elevated. Experiment 4 showed, in healthy participants, that force direction could be estimated from head motion, at levels close to the no-vision threshold for the PL participants. Our results show that proprioceptive loss influences perception, motor control and adaptation but that proprioception from the moving limb is not essential for adaptation to, or detection of, force fields. The differences in learning and retention seen between the three deafferented participants suggest that they achieve these tasks in idiosyncratic ways after proprioceptive loss, possibly integrating visual and vestibular information with individual cognitive strategies.

[1]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  David M. Huberdeau,et al.  Dual-process decomposition in human sensorimotor adaptation , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  T. Flash,et al.  Moving gracefully: quantitative theories of motor coordination , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  J. Bossom,et al.  Visuo-motor adaptation (to prismatic transformation of the retinal image) in monkeys with bilateral dorsal rhizotomy. , 1968, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[7]  S. Vrieze Model selection and psychological theory: a discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). , 2012, Psychological methods.

[8]  Jonathan Cole,et al.  Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: Studies with deafferented subjects. , 1995 .

[9]  L. Bouyer,et al.  Impact of online visual feedback on motor acquisition and retention when learning to reach in a force field , 2016, Neuroscience.

[10]  A Melendez-Calderon,et al.  Force Field Adaptation Can Be Learned Using Vision in the Absence of Proprioceptive Error , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[11]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Computational principles of sensorimotor control that minimize uncertainty and variability , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  C. Bard,et al.  Weight judgment. The discrimination capacity of a deafferented subject. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  S. Scott Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Yohsuke R. Miyamoto,et al.  Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Galea,et al.  Individual differences in explicit and implicit visuomotor learning and working memory capacity , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[19]  Á. Lago-Rodríguez,et al.  Online visual feedback during error-free channel trials leads to active unlearning of movement dynamics , 2016 .

[20]  Jean-Pierre Bresciani,et al.  Prediction in the Vestibular Control of Arm Movements. , 2015, Multisensory research.

[21]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Henriques,et al.  Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Somatosensory Plasticity and Motor Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  K. Kamijo,et al.  Fitness and ERP Indices of Cognitive Control Mode during Task Preparation in Preadolescent Children , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[29]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Fast But Fleeting: Adaptive Motor Learning Processes Associated with Aging and Cognitive Decline , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Etienne Guillaud,et al.  Prediction of the body rotation-induced torques on the arm during reaching movements: Evidence from a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  J. Lackner,et al.  Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis force perturbations of their reaching movements. , 2000, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[32]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Postural adjustments associated with different unloadings of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and cutaneous afferent deprivation. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[33]  J. Cole,et al.  Proprioception in motor learning: lessons from a deafferented subject , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  C. Bard,et al.  Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Proprioception plays a different role for sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions. , 2011, Human movement science.

[36]  Ajitkumar P. Mulavara,et al.  Individual predictors of sensorimotor adaptability , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[37]  Sarah E. Pekny,et al.  Reward-Dependent Modulation of Movement Variability , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  To transfer or not to transfer? Kinematics and laterality quotient predict interlimb transfer of motor learning. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  E. Sedgwick,et al.  The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[41]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  Spatially selective enhancement of proprioceptive acuity following motor learning. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[45]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[46]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  Erratum and Note: PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 41, 782–787 (1967)] , 1967 .

[48]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Generalization of force-field adaptation in proprioceptively-deafferented subjects , 2016, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  N Teasdale,et al.  Gait of a deafferented subject without large myelinated sensory fibers below the neck , 1996, Neurology.

[50]  Alice C. Roy,et al.  Proprioception Is Necessary for Body Schema Plasticity: Evidence from a Deafferented Patient , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[51]  R. Miall,et al.  The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions , 1967 .

[53]  G. Rees,et al.  The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  Bo Markussen,et al.  Separating Timing, Movement Conditions and Individual Differences in the Analysis of Human Movement , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[55]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[56]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[57]  M. Kawato,et al.  Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics , 2007, PloS one.

[58]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Similarities in error processing establish a link between saccade prediction at baseline and adaptation performance. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  R. Miall,et al.  Control of wrist movement in deafferented man: evidence for a mixed strategy of position and amplitude control , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  C. Bard,et al.  Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[62]  E. Vaucher,et al.  Activation of the mouse primary visual cortex by medial prefrontal subregion stimulation is not mediated by cholinergic basalo-cortical projections , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[63]  J. Vercher,et al.  Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[64]  R C Miall,et al.  System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.