Creeping dispositionism: the temporal dynamics of behavior prediction.

Four studies tested the hypothesis that temporal distance increases the weight of global dispositions in predicting and explaining future behavior. Study 1 found that the correspondence bias was manifested more strongly in predictions of distant future behavior than of near future behavior. Study 2 found that participants predicted higher cross-situational consistency in distant future behavior than in near future behavior. Study 3 found that participants sought information about others' more global dispositions for predicting distant future than near future behavior. Finally, Study 4 found that participants made more global causal attributions for distant future outcomes than for near future outcomes. The results were interpreted as supporting the assumption of construal level theory that perceivers use more abstract representations (higher level construals) to predict and explain more distant future behaviors.

[1]  D. Funder,et al.  On the nature and accuracy of attributions that change over time , 1983 .

[2]  C. Dweck,et al.  Modes of social thought: Implicit theories and social understanding. , 1999 .

[3]  V. A. Harris,et al.  The Attribution of Attitudes , 1967 .

[4]  Bill Underwood,et al.  The dispositional shift in attribution over time , 1979 .

[5]  Jerry M. Burger,et al.  Changes in Attributions Over Time: The Ephemeral Fundamental Attribution Error , 1991 .

[6]  C. Judd,et al.  Measures and models of perceived group variability , 1990 .

[7]  C. Dweck,et al.  A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality , 1988 .

[8]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[9]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[10]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The Battle of Words Between Gender Groups A Language‐Based Approach to Intergroup Processes , 1993 .

[11]  A. Tversky,et al.  Intuitive Prediction: Biases and Corrective Procedures , 1982 .

[12]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction? , 1975 .

[13]  E. E. Jones The rocky road from acts to dispositions. , 1979, The American psychologist.

[14]  Klaus Fiedler,et al.  Actor-Observer Bias in Close Relationships: The Role of Self-Knowledge and Self-Related Language , 1995 .

[15]  D. Dunning,et al.  Construal processes and trait ambiguity: implications for self-peer agreement in personality judgment. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  E. E. Jones,et al.  Perceived Variability of Personal Characteristics in In-Groups and Out-Groups , 1981 .

[17]  S. Fiske Social cognition and social perception. , 1993, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[19]  G. A. Mendelsohn,et al.  The actor-observer effect revisited: effects of individual differences and repeated social interactions on actor and observer attributions. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  L. Ross,et al.  The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  G. Caprara,et al.  The big five as tendencies in situations , 1994 .

[22]  Martin E. P. Seligman,et al.  The attributional Style Questionnaire , 1982, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[23]  L. Ross,et al.  Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by self and others. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  D. Gilbert,et al.  Focalism: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  L. Ross,et al.  Subjective Construal, Social Inference, and Human Misunderstanding , 1991 .

[26]  W. Mischel,et al.  The personality of familiar and significant people: the lay perceiver as a social-cognitive theorist. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[28]  D. Wigboldus,et al.  Children's communication of the Linguistic Intergroup Bias and its impact upon cognitive inferences , 1999 .

[29]  M. Hallahan,et al.  Explaining Real-Life Events: How Culture and Domain Shape Attributions , 1996 .

[30]  Y. Trope Identification and Inferential Processes in Dispositional Attribution. , 1986 .

[31]  Y. Trope Levels of Inference in Dispositional Judgment , 1989 .

[32]  P. Linville,et al.  The complexity–extremity effect and age-based stereotyping. , 1982 .

[33]  C. Peterson Memory and the "dispositional shift." , 1980 .

[34]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[35]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Perception of Out-Group Homogeneity and Levels of Social Categorization: Memory for the Subordinate Attributes of In-Group and Out-Group Members , 1982 .

[36]  C. Dweck,et al.  Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  W. Mischel,et al.  A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. , 1995, Psychological review.

[38]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Miswanting: Some Problems in the Forecasting of Future Affective States , 2000 .

[39]  Edward E. Jones,et al.  How do people perceive the causes of behavior , 1976 .

[40]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[41]  L. Ross,et al.  The overconfidence effect in social prediction. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  D. Sperber,et al.  Précis of Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1987 .

[43]  S. Sherman On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. , 1980 .

[44]  Y. Trope,et al.  The Use of Trait Conceptions to Identify Other People's Behavior and to Draw Inferences about their Personalities , 1993 .

[45]  L. R. Goldberg,et al.  Category breadth and hierarchical structure in personality: studies of asymmetries in judgments of trait implications. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. , 1999 .

[47]  G. Semin,et al.  Differential contributions of linguistic factors to memory based ratings: Systematizing the systematic distortion hypothesis , 1985 .

[48]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. , 2002 .

[49]  Y. Trope,et al.  Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  M. Brewer,et al.  Categorization of the Elderly by the Elderly , 1984 .

[51]  M. Morris,et al.  Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events , 1994 .

[52]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Timid choices and bold forecasts: a cognitive perspective on risk taking , 1993 .

[54]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[55]  M. Frank,et al.  Effect of memory perspective on retrospective causal attributions. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  J. S. Wiggins,et al.  The five-factor model of personality : theoretical perspectives , 1996 .

[57]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. , 1972 .

[58]  C. Judd,et al.  Role of meaningful subgroups in explaining differences in perceived variability for in-groups and out-groups. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[59]  P. Costa,et al.  Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. , 1996 .

[60]  T. Gilovich,et al.  The Perceiver as Perceived: Everyday Intuitions About the Correspondence Bias , 1999 .

[61]  E. E. Jones Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior , 1987 .

[62]  P. Costa,et al.  Personality trait structure as a human universal. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[63]  G. W. Fischer,et al.  Perceived Covariation Among the Features of Ingroup and Outgroup Members : The Outgroup Covariation Effect , 2001 .

[64]  G. Semin,et al.  Revisiting the past and back to the future: memory systems and the linguistic representation of social events. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[65]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. , 1988 .

[66]  D. Dunning A newer look: Motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. , 1999 .

[67]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[68]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Social hypothesis testing: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms. , 1996 .