Why johnny can read: the role of statistical regularities in reading and connectionist modeling
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Coltheart. Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of Reading , 1987 .
[2] J. Fodor,et al. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.
[3] R. Glushko. The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .
[4] R. Venezky. The Structure of English Orthography , 1965 .
[5] Zenon W. Pylyshyn,et al. Connectionism and cognitive architecture , 1993 .
[6] K. Forster,et al. Lexical Access and Naming Time. , 1973 .
[7] Kerry Kilborn,et al. Selective impairment of grammatical morphology due to induced stress in normal listeners: Implications for aphasia , 1991, Brain and Language.
[8] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Content and cluster analysis: Assessing representational similarity in neural systems , 2000 .
[9] Mark S. Seidenberg. The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems , 1985, Cognition.
[10] H. Kolk,et al. A Time-Based Approach to Agrammatic Production , 1995, Brain and Language.
[11] Noam Chomsky,et al. The Sound Pattern of English , 1968 .
[12] A. Friederici. The Time Course of Syntactic Activation During Language Processing: A Model Based on Neuropsychological and Neurophysiological Data , 1995, Brain and Language.
[13] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[14] D. Neary,et al. Deep Dyslexia , 1986 .
[15] Sally Andrews,et al. Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? , 1992 .
[16] V. Marchman,et al. From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets , 1993, Cognition.
[17] R. Peereman. Naming regular and exception words: Further examination of the effect of phonological dissension among lexical neighbours , 1995 .
[18] D. Plaut. Relearning after Damage in Connectionist Networks: Implications for Patient Rehabilitation , 1996 .
[19] E K Warrington,et al. Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.
[20] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[21] M. Zorzi. Serial processing in reading aloud: no challenge for a parallel model. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Organization of face and object recognition in modular neural network models , 1999, Neural Networks.
[23] Philip B. Gough,et al. One Second of Reading. , 1972 .
[24] Marco Zorzi,et al. The Development of Spelling-Sound Relationships in a Model of Phonological Reading , 1998 .
[25] T. Shallice,et al. Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .
[26] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Representing lexical semantics with context vectors and modeling lexical access with attractor networks , 1998 .
[27] John J. L. Morton,et al. Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .
[28] David C. Plaut,et al. Semantic and Associative Priming in a Distributed Attractor Network , 1995 .
[29] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Assessing the contribution of representation to results , 1998 .
[30] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999, Psychological review.
[31] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .
[32] M. Zorzi,et al. Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .
[33] Paul W. B. Atkins,et al. Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .
[34] Jeffrey L. Elman,et al. Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[35] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Image compression by back-propagation: An example of extensional programming , 1988 .
[36] K. Goodman. Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game , 1967 .
[37] Marilyn Jager Adams,et al. Models of Reading , 1982 .
[38] Gary S. Dell,et al. Structure and Content in Language Production: A Theory of Frame Constraints in Phonological Speech Errors , 1993, Cogn. Sci..
[39] Garrison W. Cottrell,et al. Acquiring the Mapping from Meaning to Sounds , 1994, Connect. Sci..
[40] Marcel Adam Just,et al. Reduced resources and specific impairments in normal and aphasic sentence comprehension , 1995 .
[41] M. Coltheart,et al. Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud , 1999 .
[42] James L. McClelland,et al. On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .
[43] S. Pinker,et al. On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.
[44] T. Bever,et al. The relation between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning—A constructive critique of some connectionist learning models , 1988, Cognition.
[45] M. Coltheart,et al. Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .
[46] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[47] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia. , 1991, Psychological review.
[48] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[49] H. Kucera,et al. Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .
[50] Ivan Illich,et al. ABC: The Alphabetization of the Popular Mind , 1988 .
[51] D. Balota,et al. The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production? ☆ , 1985 .
[52] D. Besner,et al. Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. , 1987 .