Enhancement Bias in Descriptions of Self and Others

Self-enhancement bias is the tendency to describe oneself more positively than a normative criterion would predict. This article reviews the common-rater and common-target paradigms for the assessment of enhancement bias and proposes a social-normative paradigm as an alternative. In this paradigm, enhancement bias is conceptualized as an egocentric pattern of discrepancies between self-ratings and relevant social norms. Correlations between a person's ratings of trait descriptiveness and ratings of trait desirability indicate the degree of enhancement (or diminishment) when the group averages of these ratings are controlled. Two studies demonstrate that most people self-enhance, expect others to self-enhance, and abstain from self-enhancement when instructed to estimate the social norms. Results suggest that enhancement is a controllable bias rather than a cognitive illusion.

[1]  R L Gregory Putting Illusions in their Place , 1991, Perception.

[2]  J. Block,et al.  Do positive illusions foster mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  E. Langer The illusion of control. , 1975 .

[4]  Colin Renfrew,et al.  Carbon 14 and the Prehistory of Europe , 1971 .

[5]  O. John,et al.  Accuracy and bias in self-perception: individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation of Cognitive Inference and Decision Processes , 1994 .

[7]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. , 1987 .

[8]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  D. Dunning,et al.  Idiosyncratic trait definitions: implications for self-description and social judgment. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Jonathon D. Brown,et al.  Evaluations of Self and Others: Self-Enhancement Biases in Social Judgments , 1986 .

[11]  D. Dunning,et al.  A New Look at Motivated Inference: Are Self-Serving Theories of Success a Product of Motivational Forces? , 1995 .

[12]  N. H. Berkowitz,et al.  Anticipated bias from an outgroup: An attributional analysis , 1992 .

[13]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[14]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Effects of mindset on positive illusions. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  J. Krueger Personal beliefs and cultural stereotypes about racial characteristics. , 1996 .

[16]  D. Funder,et al.  Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: negative implications for mental health. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Social categorization and memory for in-group and out-group behavior. , 1980 .

[18]  Randall A. Gordon,et al.  Social Desirability Bias: A Demonstration and Technique for Its Reduction , 1987 .

[19]  J. M. Nuttin Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect , 1985 .

[20]  D. Funder,et al.  Errors and mistakes: evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. , 1987, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  O. John,et al.  Effects of Visual Perspective and Narcissism on Self-Perception: Is Seeing Believing? , 1997 .

[22]  J. Krueger,et al.  Social categorization and the truly false consensus effect. , 1993 .

[23]  Robyn M. Dawes,et al.  An Experimental Analysis of the Contrast Effect and Its Implications for Intergroup Communication and the Indirect Assessment of Attitude. , 1972 .

[24]  T. Wills Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology , 1981 .

[25]  J. Krueger,et al.  Personality development across the adult life span: subjective conceptions vs cross-sectional contrasts. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.

[26]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[27]  L. R. Goldberg,et al.  Perceived Behavioral Consistency Underlying Trait Attributions to Oneself and Another , 1987 .

[28]  Deborah A. Prentice,et al.  Familiarity and differences in self- and other-representations. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[30]  J. Krueger,et al.  Developmental Expectations for the Self and Most Other People: Age Grading in Three Functions of Social Comparison , 1993 .

[31]  The Benefits of Illusions, the Threat of Disillusionment, and the Limitations of Inaccuracy , 1989 .

[32]  L. R. Goldberg Differential attribution of trait-descriptive terms to oneself as compared to well-liked, neutral, and disliked others: A psychometric analysis. , 1978 .

[33]  Social desirability and the description of others. , 1959, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[34]  Joachim I. Krueger,et al.  Perceptions of Behavioral Consistency: Are People Aware of the Actor-Observer Effect? , 1996 .

[35]  D. Blough,et al.  Pigeons see the Ponzo illusion , 1991 .

[36]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Do people know how others view them? An empirical and theoretical account. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  J. Krueger On the Perception of Social Consensus , 1998 .

[38]  R. Malott,et al.  The effects of outward-pointing arrowheads on the Mueller-Lyer illusion in pigeons , 1967 .

[39]  R. Thaler Toward a positive theory of consumer choice , 1980 .

[40]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Another Indirect Tactic of (Self-) Image Management , 1989 .