Yoga Lessons for Consciousness Research: A Paralimbic Network Balancing Brain Resource Allocation

Consciousness has been proposed to play a key role in shaping flexible learning and as such is thought to confer an evolutionary advantage. Attention and awareness are the perhaps most important underlying processes, yet their precise relationship is presently unclear. Both of these processes must, however, serve the evolutionary imperatives of survival and procreation. They are thus intimately bound by reward and emotion to help to prioritize efficient brain resource allocation in order to predict and optimize behavior. Here we show how this process is served by a paralimbic network consisting primarily of regions located on the midline of the human brain. Using many different techniques, experiments have demonstrated that this network is effective and specific for self-awareness and contributes to the sense of unity of consciousness by acting as a common neural path for a wide variety of conscious experiences. Interestingly, hemodynamic activity in the network decreases with focusing on external stimuli, which has led to the idea of a default mode network. This network is one of many networks that wax and vane as resources are allocated to accommodate the different cyclical needs of the organism primarily related to the fundamental pleasures afforded by evolution: food, sex, and conspecifics. Here we hypothesize, however, that the paralimbic network serves a crucial role in balancing and regulating brain resource allocation, and discuss how it can be thought of as a link between current theories of so-called “default mode,” “resting state networks,” and “global workspace.” We show how major developmental disorders of self-awareness and self-control can arise from problems in the paralimbic network as demonstrated here by the example of Asperger syndrome. We conclude that attention, awareness, and emotion are integrated by a paralimbic network that helps to efficiently allocate brain resources to optimize behavior and help survival.

[1]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[2]  F. Mayor Memory of the future , 1995 .

[3]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  T. Kjaer,et al.  Increased dopamine tone during meditation-induced change of consciousness. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  N. Schiff Central Thalamic Contributions to Arousal Regulation and Neurological Disorders of Consciousness , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Morten L. Kringelbach,et al.  Impact of Emotion on Consciousness: Positive Stimuli Enhance Conscious Reportability , 2011, PloS one.

[7]  T. Shallice,et al.  Recollection and Familiarity in Recognition Memory: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Kringelbach The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  Troels W. Kjær,et al.  Reflective Self-Awareness and Conscious States: PET Evidence for a Common Midline Parietofrontal Core , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  Anil K. Seth,et al.  Consciousness and Complexity , 2022 .

[11]  Morten L. Kringelbach,et al.  Balancing the Brain: Resting State Networks and Deep Brain Stimulation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[12]  H. Lou,et al.  Paradigm shift in consciousness research: the child′s self‐awareness and abnormalities in autism, ADHD and schizophrenia , 2012, Acta paediatrica.

[13]  W. Nauta The problem of the frontal lobe: a reinterpretation. , 1971, Journal of psychiatric research.

[14]  Katiuscia Sacco,et al.  Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[15]  Nikolaos Koutsouleris,et al.  Striatal dopamine transporter availability is associated with the productive psychotic state in first episode, drug–naive schizophrenic patients , 2006, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[16]  Andreas K. Engel,et al.  The saccadic spike artifact in MEG , 2012, NeuroImage.

[17]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  Ryusuke Kakigi,et al.  Effects of visual and auditory stimulation on somatosensory evoked magnetic fields , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  U. Frith,et al.  Brief Report: Cognitive Processing of Own Emotions in Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorder and in Their Relatives , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[21]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Connectivity-Based Parcellation of Human Cingulate Cortex and Its Relation to Functional Specialization , 2008 .

[23]  Joachim Gross,et al.  Coherence in consciousness: Paralimbic gamma synchrony of self‐reference links conscious experiences , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[24]  M. Just,et al.  Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  J. Gardiner Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a first-person approach. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[26]  F. Bermpohl,et al.  Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Monte S. Buchsbaum,et al.  Cingulate gyrus volume and metabolism in the schizophrenia spectrum , 2004, Schizophrenia Research.

[28]  E. Tulving Episodic memory: from mind to brain. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[29]  T. Kjaer,et al.  A 15O‐H2O PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[30]  C. Granger Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods , 1969 .

[31]  J. Changeux,et al.  Emergent pharmacology of conscious experience: new perspectives in substance addiction , 2011, FASEB journal : official publication of the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology.

[32]  M. Craske,et al.  A Specific and Rapid Neural Signature for Parental Instinct , 2008, PloS one.

[33]  K. Berridge,et al.  Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[34]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[35]  S Dehaene,et al.  A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  M S Buchsbaum,et al.  Limbic circuitry in patients with autism spectrum disorders studied with positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[37]  S E Petersen,et al.  Direct comparison of episodic encoding and retrieval of words: an event-related fMRI study. , 1999, Memory.

[38]  Jason W. Brown The Lost Self: Pathologies of the Brain and Identity , 2006 .

[39]  Kim Mouridsen,et al.  Dopaminergic stimulation enhances confidence and accuracy in seeing rapidly presented words. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[40]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural Correlates of First-Person Perspective as One Constituent of Human Self-Consciousness , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  E. Rolls,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[43]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[44]  H. Sackeim,et al.  Parietal cortex and representation of the mental Self. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  B. Libet Brain stimulation in the study of neuronal functions for conscious sensory experiences. , 1982, Human neurobiology.

[46]  D. Ingvar "Memory of the future": an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[47]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Emotion and consciousness , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Nikos K Logothetis,et al.  Interpreting the BOLD signal. , 2004, Annual review of physiology.

[49]  E. Maguire,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of comprehension and memory: the importance of prior knowledge. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  Joachim Gross,et al.  Recurrent Activity in Higher Order, Modality Non-Specific Brain Regions: A Granger Causality Analysis of Autobiographic Memory Retrieval , 2011, PloS one.

[51]  Alex Martin,et al.  Neural correlates of semantic and episodic memory retrieval , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Francis Eustache,et al.  Working memory and executive functions in transient global amnesia. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  T. Kjaer,et al.  Interaction between precuneus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may play a unitary role in consciousness-A principal component analysis of rCBF , 2000 .

[54]  K. Berridge,et al.  Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  S. Gallagher Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Morten L Kringelbach,et al.  Sing the mind electric – principles of deep brain stimulation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[57]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .