Unconscious Automatic Brain Activation of Acoustic and Action-related Conceptual Features during Masked Repetition Priming

Classical theories of semantic memory assume that concepts are represented in a unitary amodal memory system. In challenging this classical view, pure or hybrid modality-specific theories propose that conceptual representations are grounded in the sensory–motor brain areas, which typically process sensory and action-related information. Although neuroimaging studies provided evidence for a functional–anatomical link between conceptual processing of sensory or action-related features and the sensory–motor brain systems, it has been argued that aspects of such sensory–motor activation may not directly reflect conceptual processing but rather strategic imagery or postconceptual elaboration. In the present ERP study, we investigated masked effects of acoustic and action-related conceptual features to probe unconscious automatic conceptual processing in isolation. Subliminal feature-specific ERP effects at frontocentral electrodes were observed, which differed with regard to polarity, topography, and underlying brain electrical sources in congruency with earlier findings under conscious viewing conditions. These findings suggest that conceptual acoustic and action representations can also be unconsciously accessed, thereby excluding any postconceptual strategic processes. This study therefore further substantiates a grounding of conceptual and semantic processing in action and perception.

[1]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions , 2012, Cortex.

[2]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Spatial dynamics of masked picture repetition effects , 2007, NeuroImage.

[3]  Benjamin Naumann The Architecture Of Cognition , 2016 .

[4]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition: An Event-related Potential Investigation using Masked Repetition Priming , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain Signatures of Meaning Access in Action Word Recognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation , 2000, Neuroreport.

[8]  Jemett L. Desmond,et al.  Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  O Bertrand,et al.  A theoretical justification of the average reference in topographic evoked potential studies. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Neurophysiological distinction of verb categories , 2000, Neuroreport.

[12]  Philip M. Merikle,et al.  Measuring the Relative Magnitude of Unconscious Influences , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Tracking the Time Course of Action Priming on Object Recognition: Evidence for Fast and Slow Influences of Action on Perception , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Grainger,et al.  Exploring the temporal dynamics of visual word recognition in the masked repetition priming paradigm using event-related potentials , 2007, Brain Research.

[15]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  The modality-switch effect: visually and aurally presented prime sentences activate our senses , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[16]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Dissociating the representation of action- and sound-related concepts in middle temporal cortex , 2012, Brain and Language.

[19]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Repetition-priming Modulates Category-related Effects on Event-related Potentials: Further Evidence for Multiple Cortical Semantic Systems , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  L. Fadiga,et al.  Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding , 2012, NeuroImage.

[22]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[23]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  S Makeig,et al.  Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  M. Rugg The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[26]  T. Rogers,et al.  Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  H. Huynh,et al.  Conditions under Which Mean Square Ratios in Repeated Measurements Designs Have Exact F-Distributions , 1970 .

[28]  John R. Anderson The Architecture of Cognition , 1983 .

[29]  Olaf Hauk,et al.  Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  Ulrich Ansorge,et al.  Neuro-cognitive mechanisms of conscious and unconscious visual perception: From a plethora of phenomena to general principles , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[32]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[33]  Alex Martin,et al.  Experience-dependent modulation of category-related cortical activity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Attentional sensitization of unconscious visual processing: Top-down influences on masked priming , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[35]  Katherine A. DeLong,et al.  Close, but no garlic: Perceptuomotor and event knowledge activation during language comprehension. , 2015, Journal of memory and language.

[36]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[37]  M Spitzer,et al.  The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: evidence from event‐related potentials in a go/no go task , 1998, Neuroreport.

[38]  Ulrich Ansorge,et al.  Top-down contingent feature-specific orienting with and without awareness of the visual input , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[39]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  The Lateralization of Motor Cortex Activation to Action-Words , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  Christine D. Wilson,et al.  Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Priming of visual and functional knowledge on a semantic classification task. , 1999 .

[43]  M. Kiefer,et al.  The Sound of Concepts: Four Markers for a Link between Auditory and Conceptual Brain Systems , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[45]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Repetition priming and associative priming of face recognition: Evidence from event-related potentials. , 1995 .

[46]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Perceptual and semantic sources of category-specific effects: Event-related potentials during picture and word categorization , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[47]  A. Burton,et al.  Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  The Word Processing Deficit in Semantic Dementia: All Categories Are Equal, but Some Categories Are More Equal than Others , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation , 1996, Science.

[52]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.

[53]  Natalie M. Trumpp,et al.  Losing the sound of concepts: Damage to auditory association cortex impairs the processing of sound-related concepts , 2013, Cortex.

[54]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[55]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  H. Roediger,et al.  Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account , 1989 .

[57]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Words in the brain's language , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[59]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[60]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Priming effects in semantic classification tasks. , 1995 .

[62]  E. Warrington,et al.  Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  Sergio E. Chaigneau,et al.  THE SIMILARITY-IN-TOPOGRAPHY PRINCIPLE: RECONCILING THEORIES OF CONCEPTUAL DEFICITS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[64]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Conceptual Flexibility in the Human Brain: Dynamic Recruitment of Semantic Maps from Visual, Motor, and Motion-related Areas , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Markus Graf,et al.  The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .

[67]  Marianna D. Eddy,et al.  Masked repetition priming and event-related brain potentials: a new approach for tracking the time-course of object perception. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[68]  Alex M. Andrew,et al.  Computation and Cognition: Towards A Foundation for Cognitive Science, by Zenon W. Pylyshyn, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., xxiii + 292 pp., £26.15 , 1985, Robotica.

[69]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[70]  J. Haxby,et al.  Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[71]  Ben D. Amsel Tracking real-time neural activation of conceptual knowledge using single-trial event-related potentials , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  Edouard Machery,et al.  Concept empiricism: A methodological critique , 2007, Cognition.

[73]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Grounding the human conceptual system in perception, action, and internal states , 2013 .

[74]  Ferath Kherif,et al.  Automatic Top-Down Processing Explains Common Left Occipito-Temporal Responses to Visual Words and Objects , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[75]  L. Tyler,et al.  Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[76]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Multiple perspectives on word production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[77]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Cascade processes in picture identification , 1988 .

[78]  R. Henson Neuroimaging studies of priming , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[79]  M. Kiefer Executive control over unconscious cognition: attentional sensitization of unconscious information processing , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[80]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[81]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians , 2011, NeuroImage.

[82]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Categorizing chairs and naming pears: Category differences in object processing as a function of task and priming , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[83]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information , 1995, Nature.

[84]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Neuromagnetic evidence for early semantic access in word recognition , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[85]  T. Shallice,et al.  Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. , 2000, Science.

[86]  M. Rugg,et al.  Modality-specific effects of immediate word repetition: electrophysiological evidence. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[87]  M. Kiefer The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[88]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Masked Priming of Conceptual Features Reveals Differential Brain Activation during Unconscious Access to Conceptual Action and Sound Information , 2013, PloS one.

[89]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[90]  O. Hauk,et al.  Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto‐central cortex , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[91]  R. Henson,et al.  Priming, response learning and repetition suppression , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[92]  A. Chatterjee Disembodying cognition , 2010, Language and Cognition.

[93]  F Pulvermüller,et al.  Nouns and verbs in the intact brain: evidence from event-related potentials and high-frequency cortical responses. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[94]  B. Mesquita,et al.  Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .

[95]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[96]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[97]  Antonio R. Damasio,et al.  The Brain Binds Entities and Events by Multiregional Activation from Convergence Zones , 1989, Neural Computation.

[98]  James W. Lewis,et al.  Audio-Visual Perception of Everyday Natural Objects - Hemodynamic Studies in Humans , 2010 .

[99]  Olaf Hauk,et al.  Experience-dependent Plasticity of Conceptual Representations in Human Sensory-Motor Areas , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[100]  I. Winkler,et al.  ‘Primitive intelligence’ in the auditory cortex , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[101]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.