Song type matching is an honest early threat signal in a hierarchical animal communication system

Aggressive encounters between animals often involve significant amounts of signalling before or in lieu of physical fights. When, as is often the case, these apparent threat signals are neither inherently costly nor inherently indicative of fighting ability, we should ask whether they are in fact honest signals, i.e. do they predict that escalation is imminent? While signalling theories have indicated that such ‘conventional’ threat signals can honestly predict escalation, attempts to gather supporting empirical evidence have mostly failed. For example, recent studies in songbirds of song type matching (replying to an opponent's song with the same song type he has just sung) have failed to confirm that it predicts an eventual attack by the signaller. In the present study of song sparrows (Melospiza melodia), we tested the hypothesis that song type matching is an early threat signal in a hierarchical signalling system. We used an improved model-playback design that simulated an escalating intrusion onto the subject's territory: the simulated opponent first sang in hiding from the boundary before moving to the centre of the territory, where he revealed himself and continued to sing. We found that type matching beginning in the boundary phase and continuing into the escalation phase, or beginning immediately after the escalation, reliably predicted both subsequent escalated signalling (soft songs and wing waves) and subsequent attack on the model, supporting the hypothesis that type matching is a reliable early threat signal.

[1]  M. Nice,et al.  Studies in the Life History of the Song Sparrow II. The Behavior of the Song Sparrow and Other Passerines. , 1944 .

[2]  J. M. Smith The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. , 1974, Journal of theoretical biology.

[3]  A. Zahavi Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.

[4]  A. Zahavi The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). , 1977, Journal of theoretical biology.

[5]  R. Dawkins,et al.  Animal signals: information or manipulation? , 1978 .

[6]  J M Smith Game theory and the evolution of behaviour , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[7]  T. Clutton‐Brock,et al.  The logical stag: Adaptive aspects of fighting in red deer (Cervus elaphus L.) , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  P. G. Caryl COMMUNICATION BY AGONISTIC DISPLAYS: WHAT CAN GAMES THEORY CONTRIBUTE TO ETHOLOGY? , 1979 .

[9]  John R. Krebs,et al.  Song matching in the great tit Parus major L. , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  P. K. McGregor,et al.  Song matching in the great tit (Parus major): The effect of similarity and familiarity , 1982, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  P. G. Caryl,et al.  Communication By Agonistic Displays , 1986 .

[12]  C. Orengo,et al.  A rapid method of protein structure alignment. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[13]  A. Grafen Biological signals as handicaps. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[14]  J. Waas Do little blue penguins signal their intentions during aggressive interactions with strangers? , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  P. Stoddard,et al.  Song-type matching in the song sparrow , 1992 .

[16]  A. Grafen,et al.  Dishonesty and the handicap principle , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  M. D. Beecher,et al.  Song perception in the song sparrow: birds classify by song type but not by singer , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  T. Guilford,et al.  What are conventional signals? , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  P. Stoddard,et al.  Repertoire matching between neighbouring song sparrows , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Jack W. Bradbury,et al.  Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .

[21]  M. D. Beecher,et al.  Ecological correlates of song learning in song sparrows , 1999 .

[22]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Song type variants and aggressive context , 2000, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[23]  John M. Burt,et al.  Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  John M. Burt,et al.  Song type matching as threat: a test using interactive playback , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  P. McGregor,et al.  Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male–to–male vocal interactions , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Individual Variation in the Strength of Territory Defense in Male Song Sparrows: Correlates of Age, Territory Tenure, and Neighbor Aggressiveness , 2004 .

[27]  S. Nowicki,et al.  The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems , 2005 .

[28]  P. McGregor,et al.  Song type matching, song type switching and eavesdropping in male great tits , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[29]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Partial song matching in an eastern population of song sparrows, Melospiza melodia , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  M. D. Beecher,et al.  The role of unshared songs in singing interactions between neighbouring song sparrows , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  William A. Searcy,et al.  Bird song as a signal of aggressive intent , 2006, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[32]  Melissa Hughes,et al.  Territory owners discriminate between aggressive and nonaggressive neighbours , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Stephen P Ellner,et al.  Why Animals Lie: How Dishonesty and Belief Can Coexist in a Signaling System , 2006, The American Naturalist.

[34]  William A. Searcy,et al.  Reliable aggressive signalling in swamp sparrows , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  S. Peters,et al.  Soft Song in Song Sparrows: Acoustic Structure and Implications for Signal Function , 2008 .

[36]  M. E. Laidre,et al.  How Often Do Animals Lie about Their Intentions? An Experimental Test , 2009, The American Naturalist.

[37]  William A. Searcy,et al.  Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  Christopher N. Templeton,et al.  Indirect reciprocity: song sparrows distrust aggressive neighbours based on eavesdropping , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  Peter Marler,et al.  The Organization of Song Repertoires in Song Sparrows: Themes and Variations , 2010 .

[40]  D. Hof,et al.  Low-amplitude song predicts attack in a North American wood warbler , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  Çağlar Akçay,et al.  Sing softly and carry a big stick: signals of aggressive intent in the song sparrow , 2011, Animal Behaviour.