Spatial frequency discrimination and detection characteristics for gratings defined by orientation texture

We describe evidence consistent with the proposal that the visual system contains a parallel array of size-tuned mechanisms sensitive to orientation texture-defined (OTD) form, and propose that the relative activity of these mechanisms determines spatial frequency discrimination threshold for OTD gratings. Using a pattern of short lines we measured spatial frequency discrimination thresholds for OTD gratings and luminance-defined (LD) gratings. For OTD gratings, the orientation of texture lines varied sinusoidally across the bars of the gratings, but line luminance was constant. For LD gratings, line orientation was constant, but line luminance varied sinusoidally across the bars of the grating. When the number of texture lines (i.e. spatial samples) per grating cycle was below about six, spatial sampling strongly affected both the spatial frequency discrimination and grating detection thresholds for OTD and LD gratings. However, when the number of spatial samples per grating cycle exceeded about six, plots of both discrimination threshold and detection threshold were different for OTD and LD gratings. For an OTD grating of any given spatial frequency, spatial frequency discrimination threshold fell as the number of samples per grating cycle was increased while holding texture line length constant: the lower limit was reached at six to ten samples per cycle. When we progressively increased the viewing distance (keeping the cycles per degree (cpd) constant), spatial frequency discrimination threshold reached a lower limit and increased thereafter. We propose that this minimum threshold represents a balance between opposing effects of the number of samples per grating cycle and the length of texture lines, and approaches the absolute physiological lower limit for OTD gratings. Spatial frequency discrimination was possible up to at least 7 cpd. Grating acuity for an OTD grating was considerably lower than the physiological limit for LD gratings, presumably because detectors of OTD form include a spatial integration stage following the processing of individual lines. For an LD grating, discrimination threshold fell as the number of samples per grating cycle was increased and asymptoted at six to ten samples per cycle. Spatial frequency discrimination thresholds for OTD and LD gratings were similar at low spatial frequencies (up to 3-4 cpd), but increased more steeply for OTD gratings at high spatial frequencies. For both OTD and LD gratings, discrimination threshold fell steeply as the number of grating cycles was increased from 0.5 to ca. 2.5 cycles, and thereafter decreased more slowly or not at all suggesting that, for both OTD and LD gratings, spatial frequency discrimination can be regarded as a special case of line interval or bar width discrimination. As orientation contrast was progressively increased, discrimination threshold for an OTD grating fell steeply up to about four to five times grating detection threshold, then saturated. This parallels the effect of luminance contrast on discrimination threshold for an LD grating.

[1]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial frequency discrimination in normal vision and in patients with multiple sclerosis. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  D. Regan,et al.  Two-dimensional Aspect Ratio Discrimination for Shape Defined by Orientation Texture , 1996, Vision Research.

[3]  Hans-Christoph Nothdurft,et al.  Different approaches to the coding of visual segmentation , 1997 .

[4]  Ronald N. Bracewell,et al.  The Fourier Transform and Its Applications , 1966 .

[5]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Psychophysical isolation of movement sensitivity by removal of familiar position cues , 1981, Vision Research.

[6]  H C Nothdurft,et al.  Common properties of visual segmentation. , 1994, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[7]  B. S. Rubenstein,et al.  Spatial variability as a limiting factor in texture-discrimination tasks: implications for performance asymmetries. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  F. Kingdom,et al.  Sensitivity to orientation modulation in micropattern-based textures , 1995, Vision Research.

[9]  J. Robson Spatial and Temporal Contrast-Sensitivity Functions of the Visual System , 1966 .

[10]  David C. Burr,et al.  Local regulation of luminance gain , 1985, Vision Research.

[11]  J. Beck,et al.  Contrast and spatial variables in texture segregation: Testing a simple spatial-frequency channels model , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  F. Campbell,et al.  Spatial-frequency discrimination in human vision. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[13]  F. W. Campbell,et al.  Effect of Focus on the Visual Response to a Sinusoidally Modulated Spatial Stimulus , 1965 .

[14]  H. Nothdurft Sensitivity for structure gradient in texture discrimination tasks , 1985, Vision Research.

[15]  H. Wilson,et al.  Spatial-frequency adaptation and grating discrimination: predictions of a line-element model. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[16]  C. WILLIAM TYLER,et al.  Depth perception in disparity gratings , 1974, Nature.

[17]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[18]  D. Regan Visual information channeling in normal and disordered vision. , 1982, Psychological review.

[19]  J Hirsch,et al.  Limits of spatial-frequency discrimination as evidence of neural interpolation. , 1982, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[20]  John G. Daugman,et al.  Complete discrete 2-D Gabor transforms by neural networks for image analysis and compression , 1988, IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process..

[21]  D. Regan,et al.  Multiple sclerosis can cause visual processing deficits specific to texture-defined form. , 1995, Neurology.

[22]  T. Caelli,et al.  On the detection of Gabor signals and discrimination of Gabor textures , 1985, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Bergen,et al.  Texture segregation and orientation gradient , 1991, Vision Research.

[24]  D. H. Kelly Visual Contrast Sensitivity , 1977 .

[25]  K. Mullen The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red‐green and blue‐yellow chromatic gratings. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial-frequency discrimination and detection: comparison of postadaptation thresholds. , 1983, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[27]  H S Smallman,et al.  Fine grain of the neural representation of human spatial vision , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Regan Orientation Discrimination for Bars Defined by Orientation Texture , 1995, Perception.

[29]  T. Caelli Three processing characteristics of visual texture segmentation. , 1985, Spatial vision.

[30]  H C Nothdurft,et al.  Texton segregation by associated differences in global and local luminance distribution , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[31]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Orientation and spatial-frequency discrimination for luminance and chromatic gratings. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Combining multiple cues for texture edge localization , 1993, Electronic Imaging.

[33]  P Perona,et al.  Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[34]  Alan C. Bovik,et al.  Experiments in segmenting texton patterns using localized spatial filters , 1989, Pattern Recognit..

[35]  Jacob Beck,et al.  Spatial frequency channels and perceptual grouping in texture segregation , 1987, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[36]  H. Nothdurft The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[37]  D. Regan,et al.  Vernier step acuity and bisection acuity for texture-defined form , 1997, Vision Research.

[38]  D. Regan,et al.  Human brain electrophysiology , 1989 .

[39]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Computational and psychophysical mechanisms of visual coding , 1997 .

[40]  H. Nothdurft Orientation sensitivity and texture segmentation in patterns with different line orientation , 1985, Vision Research.

[41]  A. Y. Maudarbocus,et al.  Non-linearity of visual signals in relation to shape-sensitive adaptation responses. , 1973, Vision research.

[42]  Gregory Bock,et al.  Higher-order processing in the visual system , 1994 .

[43]  Mischa Schwartz,et al.  Information transmission, modulation, and noise , 1959 .

[44]  H. C. Nothdurft Different effects from spatial frequency masking in texture segregation and texton detection tasks , 1991, Vision Research.

[45]  E. Adelson,et al.  Early vision and texture perception , 1988, Nature.

[46]  C. Tyler Stereoscopic Vision: Cortical Limitations and a Disparity Scaling Effect , 1973, Science.

[47]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Channels in Vision: Basic Aspects , 1978 .

[48]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  J. Daugman Two-dimensional spectral analysis of cortical receptive field profiles , 1980, Vision Research.

[50]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[51]  D. Regan,et al.  Recognition and detection of texture-defined letters , 1994, Vision Research.