Failure to stop autocorrect errors in reading aloud increases in aging especially with a positive biomarker for Alzheimer's disease.

The present study examined the effects of aging and CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease (AD) on the ability to control production of unexpected words in connected speech elicited by reading aloud. Fifty-two cognitively healthy participants aged 66-86 read aloud 6 paragraphs with 10 malapropisms including 5 on content words (e.g., "window cartons" that elicited autocorrect errors to "window curtains") and 5 on function words (e.g., "thus concept" that elicited autocorrections to "this concept") and completed a battery of neuropsychological tests including a standardized Stroop task. Reading aloud elicited more autocorrect errors on function than content words, but these were equally correlated with age and Aβ1-42 levels. The ability to stop autocorrect errors declined in aging and with lower (more AD-like) levels of Aβ1-42, and multiplicatively so, such that autocorrect errors were highest in the oldest-old with the lowest Aβ1-42 levels. Critically, aging effects were significant even when controlling statistically for Aβ1-42. Finally, both autocorrect and Stroop errors were correlated with Aβ1-42, but only autocorrect errors captured unique variance in predicting Aβ1-42 levels. Reading aloud requires simultaneous planning and monitoring of upcoming speech. These results suggest that healthy aging leads to decline in the ability to intermittently monitor for and detect conflict during speech planning and that subtle cognitive changes in preclinical AD magnify this aging deficit. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).

[1]  T. Gollan,et al.  Intact Reversed Language-dominance but not Cognate Effects in Reading aloud of Language Switches in Bilingual Alzheimer’s Disease , 2020 .

[2]  K. Paterson,et al.  Older adults make greater use of word predictability in Chinese reading. , 2019, Psychology and aging.

[3]  D. Dickson,et al.  The neuropathological diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease , 2019, Molecular Neurodegeneration.

[4]  Erich S. Tusch,et al.  Markers of Novelty Processing in Older Adults Are Stable and Reliable , 2019, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[5]  A. Fagan,et al.  Developing a Spatial Navigation Screening Tool Sensitive to the Preclinical Alzheimer Disease Continuum. , 2019, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[6]  K. Blennow,et al.  Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker trajectories along the Alzheimer's disease continuum in the BIOMARKAPD study , 2019, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[7]  T. Gollan,et al.  Aging Deficits in Naturalistic Speech Production and Monitoring Revealed Through Reading Aloud , 2019, Psychology and aging.

[8]  A. Staub,et al.  Failure to detect function word repetitions and omissions in reading: Are eye movements to blame? , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Elizabeth R Schotter,et al.  What reading aloud reveals about speaking: Regressive saccades implicate a failure to monitor, not inattention, in the prevalence of intrusion errors on function words , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  Catherine Delamain,et al.  Speaking , 2018, Language for Living.

[11]  Annalena Venneri,et al.  Diagnostic and prognostic role of semantic processing in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. , 2018, Biomarkers in medicine.

[12]  D. Salmon,et al.  Bilingual language intrusions and other speech errors in Alzheimer’s disease , 2017, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  Jared M. Novick,et al.  Monitoring and Control in Language Production , 2017 .

[14]  G. Kavé,et al.  Do age-related word retrieval difficulties appear (or disappear) in connected speech? , 2017, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[15]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  The Effects of Context on Processing Words During Sentence Reading Among Adults Varying in Age and Literacy Skill , 2017, Psychology and aging.

[16]  A. Meyer,et al.  Effects of Word Frequency and Transitional Probability on Word Reading Durations of Younger and Older Speakers , 2017, Language and speech.

[17]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Association of Longitudinal Cognitive Decline With Amyloid Burden in Middle-aged and Older Adults , 2017, JAMA neurology.

[18]  Matthew W. Lowder,et al.  Effects of Word Predictability and Preview Lexicality on Eye Movements During Reading: A Comparison Between Young and Older Adults , 2017, Psychology and aging.

[19]  Douglas Galasko,et al.  NPTX2 and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s Disease , 2017, eLife.

[20]  Sarah E. Wigman,et al.  Age-Related Increases in Tip-of-the-tongue are Distinct from Decreases in Remembering Names: A Functional MRI Study , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  M. Albert,et al.  Computerized Cognitive Tests Are Associated with Biomarkers of Alzheimer’s Disease in Cognitively Normal Individuals 10 Years Prior , 2016, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[22]  T. Gollan,et al.  Grammatical Constraints on Language Switching: Language Control is not Just Executive Control. , 2016, Journal of memory and language.

[23]  J. Miller,et al.  Atypical performance patterns on Delis–Kaplan Executive Functioning System Color–Word Interference Test: Cognitive switching and learning ability in older adults , 2016, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[24]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Conflict monitoring in speech processing: An fMRI study of error detection in speech production and perception , 2016, NeuroImage.

[25]  P. Ohrmann,et al.  Linking CSF and cognition in Alzheimer's disease: Reanalysis of clinical data , 2016, Experimental Gerontology.

[26]  Chengjie Xiong,et al.  Longitudinal Cerebrospinal Fluid Biomarker Changes in Preclinical Alzheimer Disease During Middle Age. , 2015, JAMA neurology.

[27]  Michaël A. Stevens,et al.  Word knowledge in the crowd: Measuring vocabulary size and word prevalence in a massive online experiment , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Visar Berisha,et al.  Tracking discourse complexity preceding Alzheimer's disease diagnosis: a case study comparing the press conferences of Presidents Ronald Reagan and George Herbert Walker Bush. , 2015, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[29]  R. Harald Baayen,et al.  The Myth of Cognitive Decline: Non-Linear Dynamics of Lifelong Learning , 2014, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[30]  C. Jack,et al.  Biomarker Modeling of Alzheimer’s Disease , 2013, Neuron.

[31]  J. Weuve,et al.  Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010–2050) estimated using the 2010 census , 2013, Neurology.

[32]  A. Hebrank,et al.  β-Amyloid burden in healthy aging , 2012, Neurology.

[33]  Leslie M. Shaw,et al.  Standardization of preanalytical aspects of cerebrospinal fluid biomarker testing for Alzheimer's disease diagnosis: A consensus paper from the Alzheimer's Biomarkers Standardization Initiative , 2012, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[34]  Scott B. MacKenzie,et al.  Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[35]  Graeme Hirst,et al.  Longitudinal detection of dementia through lexical and syntactic changes in writing: a case study of three British novelists , 2011, Lit. Linguistic Comput..

[36]  Dean C Delis,et al.  Specific Measures of Executive Function Predict Cognitive Decline in Older Adults , 2011, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[37]  G. Dell,et al.  Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  P. Verhaeghen Aging and Executive Control: Reports of a Demise Greatly Exaggerated , 2011, Current directions in psychological science.

[39]  K. Rayner,et al.  Eye movements and word skipping during reading: effects of word length and predictability. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Charles Duyckaerts,et al.  National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association guidelines for the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer’s disease: a practical approach , 2011, Acta Neuropathologica.

[41]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension , 2010, Brain and Language.

[42]  D. Balota,et al.  The utility of Stroop task switching as a marker for early-stage Alzheimer's disease. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[43]  D. Bennett,et al.  Change in risk of Alzheimer disease over time , 2010, Neurology.

[44]  T. Salthouse Selective review of cognitive aging , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[45]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. , 2010, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[46]  Chi-Shing Tse,et al.  Predicting conversion to dementia of the Alzheimer's type in a healthy control sample: the power of errors in Stroop color naming. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[47]  R. N. Davis,et al.  Inhibition/switching is not necessarily harder than inhibition: an analysis of the D-KEFS color-word interference test. , 2010, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[48]  S. Gauthier,et al.  Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging: Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  C. Jack,et al.  Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade , 2010, The Lancet Neurology.

[50]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Beta-Amyloid Deposition and the Aging Brain , 2009, Neuropsychology Review.

[51]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t , 2009, Cognition.

[52]  A. Fagan,et al.  Multimodal techniques for diagnosis and prognosis of Alzheimer's disease , 2009, Nature.

[53]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Amyloid Deposition Is Associated with Impaired Default Network Function in Older Persons without Dementia , 2009, Neuron.

[54]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[55]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[56]  Z. Khachaturian Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association , 2008, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[57]  D. Delis,et al.  Cognitive discrepancies versus APOE genotype as predictors of cognitive decline in normal-functioning elderly individuals: a longitudinal study. , 2008, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[58]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  On the Tip-of-the-Tongue: Neural Correlates of Increased Word-finding Failures in Normal Aging , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[60]  A. Wingfield,et al.  Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[62]  D. Delis,et al.  The California Verbal Learning Test--second edition: test-retest reliability, practice effects, and reliable change indices for the standard and alternate forms. , 2006, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[63]  David A. Snowdon,et al.  Early life linguistic ability, late life cognitive function, and neuropathology: findings from the Nun Study , 2005, Neurobiology of Aging.

[64]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Aging in context: age-related changes in context use during language comprehension. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[65]  P. Verhaeghen Aging and vocabulary scores: a meta-analysis. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[66]  Douglas Galasko,et al.  Decline in verbal memory during preclinical Alzheimer's disease: Examination of the effect of APOE genotype , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[67]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .

[68]  J. Kaye,et al.  High cerebrospinal fluid tau and low amyloid beta42 levels in the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease and relation to apolipoprotein E genotype. , 1998, Archives of neurology.

[69]  D. Dahlgren,et al.  Impact of knowledge and age on tip-of-the-tongue rates. , 1998, Experimental aging research.

[70]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[71]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[72]  D R Wekstein,et al.  Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. , 1996, JAMA.

[73]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Knowledge growth and maintenance across the life span : the role of print exposure , 1995 .

[74]  L A Hansen,et al.  Clinical-neuropathological correlations in Alzheimer's disease and related dementias. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[75]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? , 1991 .

[76]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[77]  J. S. Stevenson,et al.  Older Adults , 1980, Suicide Prevention.