The role of parental cognitive, behavioral, and motor profiles in clinical variability in individuals with chromosome 16p11.2 deletions.

IMPORTANCE Most disorders caused by copy number variants (CNVs) display significant clinical variability, often referred to as incomplete penetrance and variable expressivity. Genetic and environmental sources of this variability are not well understood. OBJECTIVES To investigate the contributors to phenotypic variability in probands with CNVs involving the same genomic region; to measure the effect size for de novo mutation events; and to explore the contribution of familial background to resulting cognitive, behavioral, and motor performance outcomes in probands with de novo CNVs. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Family-based study design with a volunteer sample of 56 individuals with de novo 16p11.2 deletions and their noncarrier parents and siblings from the Simons Variation in Individuals Project. MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES We used linear mixed-model analysis to measure effect size and intraclass correlation to determine the influence of family background for a de novo CNV on quantitative traits representing the following 3 neurodevelopmental domains: cognitive ability (Full-Scale IQ), social behavior (Social Responsiveness Scale), and neuromotor performance (Purdue Pegboard Test). We included an anthropometric trait, body mass index, for comparison. RESULTS A significant deleterious effect of the 16p11.2 deletion was demonstrated across all domains. Relative to the biparental mean, the effect sizes were -1.7 SD for cognitive ability, 2.2 SD for social behavior, and -1.3 SD for neuromotor performance (P < .001). Despite large deleterious effects, significant positive correlations between parents and probands were preserved for the Full-Scale IQ (0.42 [P = .03]), the verbal IQ (0.53 [P = .004]), and the Social Responsiveness Scale (0.52 [P = .009]) scores. We also observed a 1-SD increase in the body mass index of probands compared with siblings, with an intraclass correlation of 0.40 (P = .07). CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Analysis of families with de novo CNVs provides the least confounded estimate of the effect size of the 16p11.2 deletion on heritable, quantitative traits and demonstrates a 1- to 2-SD effect across all neurodevelopmental dimensions. Significant parent-proband correlations indicate that family background contributes to the phenotypic variability seen in this and perhaps other CNV disorders and may have implications for counseling families regarding their children's developmental and psychiatric prognoses. Use of biparental mean scores rather than general population mean scores may be more relevant to examine the effect of a mutation or any other cause of trait variation on a neurodevelopmental outcome and possibly on systems of diagnosis and trait ascertainment for developmental disorders.

[1]  Raphael Bernier,et al.  The Cognitive and Behavioral Phenotype of the 16p11.2 Deletion in a Clinically Ascertained Population , 2015, Biological Psychiatry.

[2]  A. Gropman,et al.  Expanding the phenotypic profile of boys with 47, XXY: The impact of familial learning disabilities , 2014, American journal of medical genetics. Part A.

[3]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .

[4]  Adam J. Schwarz,et al.  CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls , 2013, Nature.

[5]  J. Flint,et al.  Herit-Ability , 2013, Science.

[6]  Thomas W Frazier,et al.  Commentary: The observed association between autistic severity measured by the social responsiveness scale (SRS) and general psychopathology--a response to Hus et al.(2013). , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  Vanessa Hus,et al.  Commentary: Advancing measurement of ASD severity and social competence: a reply to Constantino and Frazier (2013). , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[8]  T. Insel,et al.  Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC , 2013, BMC Medicine.

[9]  David W. Evans,et al.  Developmental brain dysfunction: revival and expansion of old concepts based on new genetic evidence , 2013, The Lancet Neurology.

[10]  Bradley P. Coe,et al.  Estimates of penetrance for recurrent pathogenic copy-number variations , 2012, Genetics in Medicine.

[11]  S. Sparrow,et al.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition , 2012 .

[12]  D H Geschwind,et al.  Using large clinical data sets to infer pathogenicity for rare copy number variants in autism cohorts , 2012, Molecular Psychiatry.

[13]  Allison G. Dempsey,et al.  A 600 kb deletion syndrome at 16p11.2 leads to energy imbalance and neuropsychiatric disorders , 2012, Journal of Medical Genetics.

[14]  Yi Zhang,et al.  Motor impairment in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for autism spectrum disorders , 2012, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[15]  The Simons,et al.  Simons Variation in Individuals Project (Simons VIP): A Genetics-First Approach to Studying Autism Spectrum and Related Neurodevelopmental Disorders , 2012, Neuron.

[16]  Ronald V. Schmelzer,et al.  Differential Abilities Scales , 2008 .

[17]  A. Sadovnick,et al.  Correlation of IQ in subjects with Down syndrome and their parents and sibs. , 2008, Journal of mental deficiency research.

[18]  Joshua M. Korn,et al.  Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. , 2008, The New England journal of medicine.

[19]  D. Conrad,et al.  Recurrent 16p11.2 microdeletions in autism. , 2007, Human molecular genetics.

[20]  J. Constantino,et al.  Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[21]  T. Crow,et al.  Familial cognitive deficits in schizophrenia , 2005, American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics.

[22]  J. Constantino,et al.  Validation of a Brief Quantitative Measure of Autistic Traits: Comparison of the Social Responsiveness Scale with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[23]  J. Constantino,et al.  Deficits in reciprocal social behavior in male twins: evidence for a genetically independent domain of psychopathology. , 2003, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[24]  R. Plomin,et al.  Genetic and environmental covariation between verbal and nonverbal cognitive development in infancy. , 2000, Child development.

[25]  L. Molinari,et al.  Phenotypic heterogeneity of growth and psychometric intelligence in Prader-Willi syndrome: variable expression of a contiguous gene syndrome or parent-child resemblance? , 2000, American journal of medical genetics.

[26]  S. Bradley-Johnson Mullen Scales of Early Learning , 1997 .

[27]  J. Kaprio,et al.  The heritability of body mass index among an international sample of monozygotic twins reared apart. , 1996, International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[28]  A. Bailey,et al.  Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study , 1995, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  C. Sultan,et al.  Study of final height in Turner's syndrome: ethnic and genetic influences. , 1994, Acta paediatrica.

[30]  G. Massa,et al.  Age and height at diagnosis in Turner syndrome: influence of parental height. , 1991, Pediatrics.

[31]  A. Jensen,et al.  Heritability of IQ. , 1976, Science.

[32]  A. Prader,et al.  GROWTH IN CHILDREN WITH 45 XO TURNER'S SYNDROME , 1975, Pediatric Research.

[33]  David W. Smith,et al.  THE XO SYNDROME. A STUDY OF THE DIFFERENTIATED PHENOTYPE IN 25 PATIENTS. , 1963, The Journal of pediatrics.

[34]  J. Tiffin,et al.  The Purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. , 1948, The Journal of applied psychology.

[35]  R. Holl,et al.  Turner syndrome: Final height, glucose tolerance, bone density and psychosocial status in 25 adult patients , 2005, European Journal of Pediatrics.

[36]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Intelligence: genetics, genes, and genomics. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  K. Sundet,et al.  PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF A NORWEGIAN RESEARCH VERSION OF THE 'WECHSLER ABBREVIATED SCALE OF INTELLIGENCE' (WASI) , 2001 .

[38]  M. Preece,et al.  The genetic contribution to stature. , 1996, Hormone research.

[39]  T. Gasser,et al.  Height correlations between parents and mature offspring in normal subjects and in subjects with Turner's and Klinefelter's and other syndromes. , 1977, Annals of human biology.