Social rank and cortisol among female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).

In animal societies, some stressful events can lead to higher levels of physiological stress. Such stressors, like social rank, also predict an increased vulnerability to an array of diseases. However, the physiological relationship between social rank and stress varies between different species, as well as within groups of a single species. For example, dominant individuals are more socially stressed at times, while at other times it is the subordinate ones who experience this stress. Together, these variations make it difficult to assess disease vulnerability as connected to social interactions. In order to learn more about how physiological rank relationships vary between groups of a single species, cortisol measurements from hair samples were used to evaluate the effects of dominance rank on long-term stress levels in despotic and less stringent female rhesus macaque hierarchal groups. In despotic groups, cortisol levels were found not to be correlated with social rank, but a negative correlation was found between social rank and cortisol levels in less stringent hierarchies. Low ranking monkeys in less stringent groups secreted elevated levels of cortisol compared to higher ranking animals. These data suggest that variations in the strictness of the dominance hierarchy are determining factors in rank related stress physiology. The further consideration of nonhuman primate social system diversity and the linear degree of their hierarchies may allow for the development of valid rank-related stress models that will help increase our understanding and guide the development of new therapeutics for diseases related to human socioeconomic status.

[1]  C. Shively,et al.  Modeling depression in adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 2012, American journal of primatology.

[2]  Evan Russell,et al.  Hair cortisol as a biological marker of chronic stress: Current status, future directions and unanswered questions , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[3]  Yuanye Ma,et al.  Maternal separation produces lasting changes in cortisol and behavior in rhesus monkeys , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  G. Koren,et al.  An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical applications. , 2010, Forensic science international.

[5]  Giorgia Antonelli,et al.  Cortisol assays and diagnostic laboratory procedures in human biological fluids. , 2009, Clinical biochemistry.

[6]  I. Veissier,et al.  Exploration of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal function as a tool to evaluate animal welfare , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  T. Kaur,et al.  FECAL GLUCOCORTICOIDS AND THEIR METABOLITES AS INDICATORS OF STRESS IN VARIOUS MAMMALIAN SPECIES: A LITERATURE REVIEW , 2006, Journal of zoo and wildlife medicine : official publication of the American Association of Zoo Veterinarians.

[8]  M. Novak,et al.  Analysis of endogenous cortisol concentrations in the hair of rhesus macaques. , 2006, General and comparative endocrinology.

[9]  T. Rogers,et al.  Urinary cortisol sampling: a non‐invasive technique for examining cortisol concentrations in the Weddell seal, Leptonychotes weddellii , 2006 .

[10]  M. Owen,et al.  Seasonal and diurnal dynamics of glucocorticoids and behavior in giant pandas , 2005 .

[11]  R. Sapolsky The Influence of Social Hierarchy on Primate Health , 2005, Science.

[12]  David P. Friedman,et al.  Social stress-associated depression in adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) , 2005, Biological Psychology.

[13]  R. Sapolsky Social Status and Health in Humans and Other Animals , 2004 .

[14]  S. Peter Henzi,et al.  Fecal cortisol levels in free-ranging female chacma baboons: relationship to dominance, reproductive state and environmental factors , 2004, Hormones and Behavior.

[15]  Michael Marmot,et al.  Health inequalities and the psychosocial environment-two scientific challenges. , 2004, Social science & medicine.

[16]  Han de Vries,et al.  David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.'s index , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  G. Stenhouse,et al.  PHYSIOLOGIC RESPONSES OF GRIZZLY BEARS TO DIFFERENT METHODS OF CAPTURE , 2003, Journal of wildlife diseases.

[18]  Mewa Singh,et al.  Methodological considerations in measurement of dominance in primates , 2003 .

[19]  E. Keverne,et al.  Are subordinates always stressed? a comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates , 2003, Hormones and Behavior.

[20]  J. Mackenbach Mind the gap--hierarchies, health and human evolution. , 2002, International journal of epidemiology.

[21]  Craig Tovey,et al.  Individual differences versus social dynamics in the formation of animal dominance hierarchies , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  R. Wilkinson Mind the Gap: Hierarchies, Health, and Human Evolution , 2000 .

[23]  E. Epel,et al.  Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. , 2000, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[24]  R. Sapolsky,et al.  How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. , 2000, Endocrine reviews.

[25]  R. Wennig Potential problems with the interpretation of hair analysis results. , 2000, Forensic science international.

[26]  S. Cavigelli Behavioural patterns associated with faecal cortisol levels in free-ranging female ring-tailed lemurs,Lemur catta , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  F. Dhabhar,et al.  Enhancing versus suppressive effects of stress hormones on skin immune function. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  C. Shively,et al.  Social subordination stress, behavior, and central monoaminergic function in female cynomolgus monkeys , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  D. Abbott,et al.  Suppression of Cortisol Levels in Subordinate Female Marmosets: Reproductive and Social Contributions , 1998, Hormones and Behavior.

[30]  M. Williams,et al.  How to do it: Run a preparation course for postgraduate examinations , 1997 .

[31]  S. Manuck,et al.  Chronic Social Stress, Social Status, and Susceptibility to Upper Respiratory Infections in Nonhuman Primates , 1997, Psychosomatic medicine.

[32]  C. Shively,et al.  Behavior and physiology of social stress and depression in female cynomolgus monkeys , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  G. Cornelissen,et al.  The circadian rhythm of cortisol in the saliva of young pigs , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  The Use of Saliva Cortisol, Urinary Cortisol, and Catecholamine Measurements for a Noninvasive Assessment of Stress Responses in Dogs , 1996, Hormones and Behavior.

[35]  F. Bercovitch,et al.  Dominance rank, cortisol concentrations, and reproductive maturation in male rhesus macaques , 1995, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  S. Manuck,et al.  The pathogenicity of behavior and its neuroendocrine mediation: an example from coronary artery disease. , 1995, Psychosomatic medicine.

[37]  D. Abbott,et al.  Social and reproductive influences on plasma cortisol in female marmoset monkeys , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[38]  C. Shively,et al.  Social status and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female monkeys. , 1994, Arteriosclerosis and thrombosis : a journal of vascular biology.

[39]  T. P. Gordon,et al.  Relationship between Social Factors and Pituitary-Adrenocortical Activity in Female Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) , 1993, Hormones and Behavior.

[40]  Mewa Singh,et al.  Hierarchy, kinship and social interaction among Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) , 1992, Journal of Biosciences.

[41]  H. McClure,et al.  Formation of a new social group of unfamiliar female rhesus monkeys affects the immune and pituitary adrenocortical systems , 1991, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[42]  M. Blankenstein,et al.  A pilot study of the social correlates of levels of urinary cortisol, prolactin, and testosterone in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) , 1991, Primates.

[43]  W. Mason,et al.  Breeding readiness in squirrel monkeys: Female-primed females are triggered by males , 1991, Physiology & Behavior.

[44]  R. Sapolsky Adrenocortical function, social rank, and personality among wild baboons , 1990, Biological Psychiatry.

[45]  M. Perret,et al.  Chemical signals and reproductive capactiy in a male prosimian primate (Microcebus murinus) , 1987 .

[46]  E. Keverne,et al.  Relationship between dominance hierarchy, cerebrospinal fluid levels of amine transmitter metabolites (5-hydroxyindole acetic acid and homovanillic acid) and plasma cortisol in monkeys , 1985, Neuroscience.

[47]  M. Adams,et al.  Psychosocial influences on ovarian endocrine and ovulatory function in Macaca fascicularis , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[48]  C. Snowdon,et al.  A breeding colony of cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). , 1985, Laboratory animal science.

[49]  R F Vining,et al.  Salivary Cortisol: A Better Measure of Adrenal Cortical Function than Serum Cortisol , 1983, Annals of clinical biochemistry.

[50]  W. Burch Urine free-cortisol determination. A useful tool in the management of chronic hypoadrenal states. , 1982, JAMA.

[51]  C. Coe,et al.  Social status constrains the stress response in the squirrel monkey , 1979, Physiology & Behavior.

[52]  C. Coe,et al.  The physiological response to group formation in adult male squirrel monkeys , 1978, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[53]  A. Leshner,et al.  Dominance status and adrenocortical reactivity to stress in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) , 1975, Primates.

[54]  Wang Xiao-wei The Methods of Research on the Dominance of Female in Non-human Primates , 2011 .

[55]  Z. Yuhui,et al.  Review on Dominance Hierarchy of Non-human Primates , 2004 .

[56]  A. Birn The Health of Nations: Why Inequality Is Harmful to Your Health , 2003 .

[57]  Christopher Servheen,et al.  Measuring stress in mammals using fecal glucocorticoids: opportunities and challenges , 2002 .

[58]  B. McEwen,et al.  The end of stress as we know it , 2002 .

[59]  J. Millspaugh,et al.  Non-invasive techniques for stress assessment in white-tailed deer , 2002 .

[60]  B. Wallner,et al.  Fecal glucocorticoids document stress in female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). , 1999, General and comparative endocrinology.

[61]  P. Whitten,et al.  Response of fecal cortisol to stress in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 1998, American journal of primatology.

[62]  R. Sapolsky,et al.  Rank‐related differences in cardiovascular function among wild baboons: Role of sensitivity to glucocorticoids , 1994, American journal of primatology.

[63]  Robert M. Sapolsky,et al.  Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers , 1994 .

[64]  H. D. Steklis,et al.  Biochemical and hormonal correlates of dominance and social behavior in all‐male groups of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) , 1986, American journal of primatology.

[65]  E. Keverne,et al.  Dominance and Subordination: Concepts or Physiological States? , 1982 .

[66]  A. Chamove,et al.  Rank, rhesus social behavior, and stress. , 1976, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[67]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.