Processing of retinal and extraretinal signals for memory-guided saccades during smooth pursuit.

It is an essential feature for the visual system to keep track of self-motion to maintain space constancy. Therefore the saccadic system uses extraretinal information about previous saccades to update the internal representation of memorized targets, an ability that has been identified in behavioral and electrophysiological studies. However, a smooth eye movement induced in the latency period of a memory-guided saccade yielded contradictory results. Indeed some studies described spatially accurate saccades, whereas others reported retinal coding of saccades. Today, it is still unclear how the saccadic system keeps track of smooth eye movements in the absence of vision. Here, we developed an original two-dimensional behavioral paradigm to further investigate how smooth eye displacements could be compensated to ensure space constancy. Human subjects were required to pursue a moving target and to orient their eyes toward the memorized position of a briefly presented second target (flash) once it appeared. The analysis of the first orientation saccade revealed a bimodal latency distribution related to two different saccade programming strategies. Short-latency (<175 ms) saccades were coded using the only available retinal information, i.e., position error. In addition to position error, longer-latency (>175 ms) saccades used extraretinal information about the smooth eye displacement during the latency period to program spatially more accurate saccades. Sensory parameters at the moment of the flash (retinal position error and eye velocity) influenced the choice between both strategies. We hypothesize that this tradeoff between speed and accuracy of the saccadic response reveals the presence of two coupled neural pathways for saccadic programming. A fast striatal-collicular pathway might only use retinal information about the flash location to program the first saccade. The slower pathway could involve the posterior parietal cortex to update the internal representation of the flash once extraretinal smooth eye displacement information becomes available to the system.

[1]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[2]  K Ohtsuka Properties of memory-guided saccades toward targets flashed during smooth pursuit in human subjects. , 1994, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  T. Hodgson,et al.  The location marker effect. Saccadic latency increases with target eccentricity. , 2002, Experimental brain research.

[4]  M. Missal,et al.  Quantitative analysis of catch-up saccades during sustained pursuit. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  T Vieville,et al.  Use of target velocity in saccadic programming. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R J Leigh,et al.  Two distinct deficits of visual tracking caused by unilateral lesions of cerebral cortex in humans , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[8]  N J Gandhi,et al.  Discharge of superior collicular neurons during saccades made to moving targets. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Natural and drug-induced variations of velocity and duration of human saccadic eye movements: Evidence for a control of the neural pulse generator by local feedback , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[10]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Tracking with the mind’s eye , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  R. Leigh,et al.  The neurology of eye movements , 1984 .

[12]  D Guitton,et al.  Human head-free gaze saccades to targets flashed before gaze-pursuit are spatially accurate. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Eye position effects in monkey cortex. II. Pursuit- and fixation-related activity in posterior parietal areas LIP and 7A. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  L. Stark,et al.  Role of corollary discharge in space constancy , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[16]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[17]  B. Bridgeman A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems , 1995, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[18]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Retinal eccentricity and the latency of eye saccades , 1994, Vision Research.

[19]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[20]  D. A. Suzuki,et al.  Smooth-pursuit eye movement deficits with chemical lesions in the dorsolateral pontine nucleus of the monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[22]  R. Gellman,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  Spatial memory following shifts of gaze. I. Saccades to memorized world-fixed and gaze-fixed targets. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Marc A Sommer,et al.  The role of the thalamus in motor control , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  S. Gielen,et al.  A quantitative analysis of generation of saccadic eye movements by burst neurons. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J Tanji,et al.  Microstimulation of the lateral wall of the intraparietal sulcus compared with the frontal eye field during oculomotor tasks. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  A J Van Opstal,et al.  Local feedback signals are not distorted by prior eye movements: evidence from visually evoked double saccades. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  B. Fischer,et al.  Separate populations of visually guided saccades in humans: reaction times and amplitudes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Properties of signals that determine the amplitude and direction of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[31]  E. Maylor,et al.  Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  D. Zee,et al.  Oculomotor function in the rhesus monkey after deafferentation of the extraocular muscles , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[34]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Saccades can be aimed at the spatial location of targets flashed during pursuit. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Role of the rostral superior colliculus in active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Monkey posterior parietal cortex neurons antidromically activated from superior colliculus. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Interaction between smooth anticipation and saccades during ocular orientation in darkness. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Timothy L. Hodgson,et al.  The location marker effect , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Latency of saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movement in man Directional asymmetries , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  R. Wurtz,et al.  What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[43]  E. Keller,et al.  Velocity prediction in corrective saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  B A J Reddi,et al.  Accuracy, information, and response time in a saccadic decision task. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  James J. Clark Spatial attention and latencies of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Vision Research.

[46]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[47]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  W. Heide,et al.  Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[49]  N. Tobler,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(00)00316-3 , 2001 .

[50]  Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al.  Testing quasi-visual neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field with the triple-step paradigm , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  S de Brouwer,et al.  Role of retinal slip in the prediction of target motion during smooth and saccadic pursuit. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  John R. Williams DECLARATION OF HELSINKI , 1964 .

[53]  Lennerstrand Advances in Strabismus Research: Basic and Clinical Aspects , 2001 .

[54]  D. Munoz,et al.  Influence of stimulus eccentricity and direction on characteristics of pro- and antisaccades in non-human primates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Prediction of saccadic amplitude during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2000 .

[56]  L F Dell'Osso,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  C. Weir,et al.  Spatial localisation: does extraocular muscle proprioception play a role? , 2000, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[58]  W. A. Fletcher,et al.  Eye position signals in human saccadic processing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  R. Kanai,et al.  Inhibition of saccade initiation by preceding smooth pursuit , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements during the visually induced sensation of ego-motion in humans , 1997, Vision Research.

[61]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Colliding saccades evoked by frontal eye field stimulation: artifact or evidence for an oculomotor compensatory mechanism underlying double-step saccades? , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[62]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[63]  R. Wurtz,et al.  What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. I. Oculomotor signals sent from superior colliculus to frontal eye field via mediodorsal thalamus. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  R. Aslin,et al.  The amplitude and angle of saccades to double-step target displacements , 1987, Vision Research.

[65]  J Schlag,et al.  Interactions between natural and electrically evoked saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: effects of randomization and daily practice , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  B. Fischer,et al.  The analysis of saccadic eye movements from gap and overlap paradigms. , 1997, Brain research. Brain research protocols.

[68]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[69]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[71]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  The frontal eye field provides the goal of saccadic eye movement , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets , 1976, Vision Research.

[73]  Dottie M. Clower,et al.  The Inferior Parietal Lobule Is the Target of Output from the Superior Colliculus, Hippocampus, and Cerebellum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[74]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[75]  J. Reulen,et al.  Latency of visually evoked saccadic eye movements , 1984, Biological Cybernetics.

[76]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[77]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Colliding saccades may reveal the secret of their marching orders , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[78]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.