Don't stand so close to me: investigating the effect of control on the appeal of virtual humans using immersion and a proximity-based behavioral task

In recent years, there has been much research and media attention devoted to investigating virtual reality environments. In this paper, we are investigating if there are differences in how characters are perceived in immersive virtual reality as opposed to more common, screen-based environments. We were particularly interested if the spatial and immersive components play an important part in perception of interactive, game-like settings, where characters can either be controlled (avatars) or observed (agents). We focus on the subjective reports on perceived realism, affinity, co-presence and agency. Since appearance of the character is an important component of affinity, we introduced the changes in render style, ranging in three realism levels, to test if appearance would even further influence the perception in relation to control condition and platform. Furthermore, we adapted a behavioural method (proximity task) as a novel approach to establishing if behavioural changes could be recorded based on the introduced conditions and compared those values with the subjective reports of the participants. The conclusions have an important value to character design specific to platform and character control.

[1]  Angela Tinwell,et al.  Perception of psychopathy and the Uncanny Valley in virtual characters , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[2]  Qiang Fu,et al.  The effect of avatar model in stepping off a ledge in an immersive virtual environment , 2015, SAP.

[3]  Jun'ichiro Seyama,et al.  The Uncanny Valley: Effect of Realism on the Impression of Artificial Human Faces , 2007, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[4]  Mel Slater,et al.  The impact of avatar realism and eye gaze control on perceived quality of communication in a shared immersive virtual environment , 2003, CHI '03.

[5]  E. Hall,et al.  The Hidden Dimension , 1970 .

[6]  Jessica K. Hodgins,et al.  The saliency of anomalies in animated human characters , 2010, TAP.

[7]  Jessica K. Hodgins,et al.  Perceptual effects of damped and exaggerated facial motion in animated characters , 2013, 2013 10th IEEE International Conference and Workshops on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition (FG).

[8]  H. Ishiguro,et al.  The thing that should not be: predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving human and humanoid robot actions , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[9]  Susan Persky,et al.  The Independent and Interactive Effects of Embodied-Agent Appearance and Behavior on Self-Report, Cognitive, and Behavioral Markers of Copresence in Immersive Virtual Environments , 2005, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[10]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  J. Bailenson,et al.  Comparing Behavioral and Self-Report Measures of Embodied Agents' Social Presence in Immersive Virtual Environments , 2004 .

[12]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  The Effect of Viewing a Self-Avatar on Distance Judgments in an HMD-Based Virtual Environment , 2010, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[13]  Tom Geller,et al.  Overcoming the Uncanny Valley , 2008, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.

[14]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[15]  Frank Biocca,et al.  The Cyborg's Dilemma: Progressive Embodiment in Virtual Environments , 2006, J. Comput. Mediat. Commun..

[16]  Rachel McDonnell,et al.  Animation realism affects perceived character appeal of a self-virtual face , 2015, MIG.

[17]  Mel Slater,et al.  Proxemics with multiple dynamic characters in an immersive virtual environment , 2010, TAP.

[18]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Render me real? , 2012, ACM Trans. Graph..

[19]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  Equilibrium Theory Revisited: Mutual Gaze and Personal Space in Virtual Environments , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[20]  Karl F. MacDorman,et al.  Does Japan really have robot mania? Comparing attitudes by implicit and explicit measures , 2008, AI & SOCIETY.

[21]  G. Evans,et al.  Personal space. , 1973, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Mel Slater,et al.  Meeting people vitually: experiments in shared virtual environments , 2002 .

[23]  J. Kätsyri,et al.  A review of empirical evidence on different uncanny valley hypotheses: support for perceptual mismatch as one road to the valley of eeriness , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[24]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Jessica K. Hodgins,et al.  Unpleasantness of animated characters corresponds to increased viewer attention to faces , 2013, SAP.

[26]  Diego Gutierrez,et al.  To stylize or not to stylize? , 2015, ACM Trans. Graph..

[27]  J. Loomis,et al.  Interpersonal Distance in Immersive Virtual Environments , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[28]  J. Hodgins,et al.  Anthropomorphism influences perception of computer-animated characters' actions. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  Philip A. Kragel,et al.  Hemodynamic signals of mixed messages during a social exchange , 2011, Neuroreport.

[30]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  The Proteus Effect , 2009, Commun. Res..