Brain-behavior relations in reading and dyslexia: Implications of Chinese results

It is ironic that our recent report on the neural correlatesof Chinese dyslexia (Siok, Perfetti, Jin, & Tan, 2004) raisesa concern by Ziegler (2006) that this report could under-mine an agreed-upon conclusion favoring a phonologicaldeficit as the cause of reading disability. Our past researchover 20 years in English (Perfetti, 1985) and Chinese (Per-fetti, Liu, & Tan, 2005; Pollatsek, Tan, & Rayner, 2000;Tan & Perfetti, 1998) has argued that phonological pro-cesses are intrinsic to word reading and universal acrosswriting systems, and we certainly have no reason to mini-mize the importance of these processes for understandingreading disability. We address Ziegler’s concern by makingtwo observations. (1) The Siok et al. evidence supporting arole for the left middle frontal gyrus (LMFG) instead ofthe left posterior temporoparietal regions found in alpha-betic research does not undermine the consensus on theuniversality of phonology. Instead this evidence shows thatthe implementation of phonology depends on language andwriting systems, a conclusion also reached through behav-ioral evidence (Perfetti et al., 2005). (2) The detailed neuralbasis of dyslexia remains an open question, rather thanaccepted wisdom.On the first point, Siok et al. did not conclude that pho-nology plays no role in Chinese reading disability and Zie-gler’s critique recognizes this. Indeed, the evidence in favorof a phonological basis for dyslexia in alphabetic writinghas accumulated impressively over 20 years, from a timeat which the prevailing views emphasized visual problemsas the cause of dyslexia. This evidence comes from neuro-biological (Brunswick, McCrory, Price, Frith, & Frith,1999; Eden et al., 2004; Price & Mechelli, 2005; Richards,Aylward, & Berninger, 2006; Shaywitz et al., 1998; Templeet al., 2003), cognitive (Bruck, 1992; Schatschneider,Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004; Stanovich& Siegel, 1994; Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon,2004), and genetic perspectives (Taipale et al., 2003). Themore biologically specific form of the phonological hypoth-esis is that dyslexia is universally associated with functionaldisruption of the left temporoparietal brain regions(Paulesu et al., 2001). It is this hypothesis that is falsifiedby Siok et al. (2004).To summarize briefly, using homophone and lexicaldecision tasks, we found that reading difficulty in Chi-nese is associated not only with a weakness in mappingof a character’s orthography to pronunciation (as mea-sured by the homophone decision task), but also witha substandard connection between orthography andmeaning (as indexed by the lexical decision paradigm).Importantly, these two deficits were characterized by dys-function of the left middle frontal gyrus. The left tempo-roparietal brain regions associated with disabled readingin English and other alphabets were not involved in thereading of Chinese children. Thus, either Chinese readingdisability does not have a phonological component orthe phonological component in Chinese does not involvethe left temporoparietal regions. Because the Siok et al.behavioral data showed weakness in phonological pro-cessing, the conclusion is that this weakness is associatedwith some other neural substrate. The left middle frontalgyrus, which was under activated in Chinese dyslexics, isa likely candidate.However, this is not the end of the story, but the begin-ning. The research goal becomes to understand the func-tion of the LMFG in reading. It is not obvious that theLMFG, whatever its more general role in cognitive func-

[1]  A. Mechelli,et al.  Reading and reading disturbance , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  L. Tan,et al.  Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture , 2004, Nature.

[3]  L. Tan,et al.  Distinct brain regions associated with syllable and phoneme , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[4]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  Ying Liu,et al.  Orthography to Phonology and Meaning: Comparisons Across and within Writing Systems , 2005 .

[6]  A. Liberman,et al.  Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  U. Frith,et al.  Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  U. Frith,et al.  Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics , 1999 .

[10]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. , 1994 .

[11]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[12]  W. Nagy,et al.  Individual fMRI activation in orthographic mapping and morpheme mapping after orthographic or morphological spelling treatment in child dyslexics , 2006, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[13]  Karen M. Jones,et al.  Neural Changes following Remediation in Adult Developmental Dyslexia , 2004, Neuron.

[14]  Kenneth R. Pugh,et al.  Examining Reading Development and Reading Disability in English Language Learners: Potential Contributions from Functional Neuroimaging , 2005 .

[15]  L. Tan,et al.  Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  A Pollatsek,et al.  The role of phonological codes in integrating information across saccadic eye movements in Chinese character identification. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  Maggie Bruck,et al.  Persistence of dyslexics' phonological deficits. , 1992 .

[19]  L. Tan,et al.  Phonological codes as early sources of constraint in Chinese word identification: A review of current discoveries and theoretical accounts , 1998 .

[20]  Jack M. Fletcher,et al.  Kindergarten Prediction of Reading Skills: A Longitudinal Comparative Analysis , 2004 .

[21]  J. B. Wyman,et al.  What is reading ability , 1921 .

[22]  Johannes C. Ziegler,et al.  Do differences in brain activation challenge universal theories of dyslexia? , 2006, Brain and Language.

[23]  M. Bruck Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. , 1992 .

[24]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[25]  P. Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Ying Liu,et al.  The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.

[27]  F. Fazio,et al.  Dyslexia: Cultural Diversity and Biological Unity , 2001, Science.