Orienting of Attention to Threatening Facial Expressions Presented under Conditions of Restricted Awareness

Three studies investigated whether individuals preferentially allocate attention to the spatial location of threatening faces presented outside awareness. Pairs of face stimuli were briefly displayed and masked in a modified version of the dot-probe task. Each face pair consisted of an emotional (threat or happy) and neutral face. The hypothesis that preattentive processing of threat results in attention being oriented towards its location was supported in Experiments 1 and 3. In both studies, this effect was most apparent in the left visual field, suggestive of right hemisphere involvement. However, in Experiment 2 where awareness of the faces was less restricted (i.e. marginal threshold conditions), preattentive capture of attention by threat was not evident. There was evidence from Experiment 3 that the tendency to orient attention towards masked threat faces was greater in high than low trait anxious individuals.

[1]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety , 2000 .

[2]  K. Mogg,et al.  Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: a replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. , 1999, Behaviour research and therapy.

[3]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional Bias for Threatening Facial Expressions in Anxiety: Manipulation of Stimulus Duration , 1998 .

[4]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[6]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Lateralization in Emotion and Emotional Disorders , 1998 .

[7]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[8]  K. Mogg,et al.  Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[9]  D. Williamson,et al.  Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in depression. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[10]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Tachistoscopic applications of Micro Experimental Laboratory (MEL) used with IBM PC compatibles: Stimulus and response timing issues , 1995 .

[12]  Michael W. Eysenck,et al.  Trait anxiety, anxious mood, and threat detection. , 1995 .

[13]  R. McNally,et al.  Automaticity and the anxiety disorders. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[14]  Murray White,et al.  Preattentive analysis of facial expressions of emotion , 1995 .

[15]  K. Mogg,et al.  Selective processing of negative information: effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[16]  M. Kindt,et al.  Responding to subliminal threat cues is related to trait anxiety and emotional vulnerability: a successful replication of Macleod and Hagan (1992) , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[17]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Activation by marginally perceptible ("subliminal") stimuli: dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[19]  Karin Mogg,et al.  Attentional Bias to Threat: Roles of Trait Anxiety, Stressful Events, and Awareness , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  A. Öhman,et al.  Automatically elicited fear: Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions , 1994 .

[21]  A. Ohman,et al.  "Unconscious anxiety": phobic responses to masked stimuli. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  K. Mogg,et al.  Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[24]  E. Fox,et al.  Allocation of visual attention and anxiety. , 1993, Cognition & emotion.

[25]  C MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  M. Eysenck Anxiety: The Cognitive Perspective , 1992 .

[27]  C MacLeod,et al.  Individual differences in the selective processing of threatening information, and emotional responses to a stressful life event. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[28]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  J. Williams Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders , 1991 .

[30]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: Conscious influences on unconscious perception. , 1989 .

[32]  D. Purcell,et al.  Probing "pop-out": Another look at the face-in-the-crowd effect. , 1989 .

[33]  Murray Alpert,et al.  Facial asymmetry while posing positive and negative emotions: Support for the right hemisphere hypothesis , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  A. Starr,et al.  MODALITY-SPECIFIC EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL CORRELATES OF THE SERIAL POSITION EFFECT IN SHORT-TERM-MEMORY , 1988 .

[35]  Donald E. Broadbent,et al.  Anxiety and Attentional Bias: State and Trait , 1988 .

[36]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd : An anger superiority effect , 1988 .

[37]  P. Merikle,et al.  Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.

[38]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Attentional bias in emotional disorders. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[39]  G. Rhodes Lateralized processes in face recognition. , 1985, British journal of psychology.

[40]  David Navon,et al.  Allocation of Attention According to Informativeness in Visual Recognition , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[41]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Differential contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres to the perception of happy and sad faces , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  R. Friend,et al.  Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. , 1969, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[45]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[46]  K. Mogg,et al.  ATTENTIONAL BIASES FOR EMOTIONAL FACES , 1997 .

[47]  Joseph E LeDoux The emotional brain , 1996 .

[48]  E. Fox Selective Processing of Threatening Words in Anxiety: The Role of Awareness , 1996 .

[49]  Joseph E LeDoux Emotion: clues from the brain. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[50]  Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al.  Left/right and cortical/subcortical dichotomies in the neuropsychological study of human emotions , 1993 .

[51]  Arne Öhman,et al.  Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms. , 1993 .

[52]  R. Davidson Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and methodological conundrums , 1993 .

[53]  A. Mathews Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[54]  M. Lorr,et al.  Manual for the Profile of Mood States , 1971 .

[55]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .