Impaired disengagement from threatening cues of impending pain in a crossmodal cueing paradigm

[1]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[2]  Léon Plaghki,et al.  Nociceptive processing in the human brain of infrequent task-relevant and task-irrelevant noxious stimuli. A study with event-related potentials evoked by CO2 laser radiant heat stimuli , 2003, Pain.

[3]  S. Minoshima,et al.  Keeping pain out of mind: the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in pain modulation. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  G. Crombez,et al.  Retarded disengagement from pain cues: the effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy , 2002, Pain.

[5]  G. Crombez,et al.  Exposure to physical movements in low back pain patients: restricted effects of generalization. , 2002, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[6]  C. Spence Multisensory attention and tactile information-processing , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  M. Zeegers,et al.  The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain‐related stimuli among chronic pain patients: a meta‐analysis , 2002, European journal of pain.

[8]  J. Vlaeyen,et al.  The Treatment of Fear of Movement/(Re)injury in Chronic Low Back Pain: Further Evidence on the Effectiveness of Exposure In Vivo , 2002, The Clinical journal of pain.

[9]  Charles Spence,et al.  Selective attention to pain: a psychophysical investigation , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  R. Dowman Pain-evoked anterior cingulate activity generating the negative difference potential may reflect response selection processes. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[11]  E. Fox,et al.  Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces , 2002, Cognition & emotion.

[12]  G. Crombez,et al.  Exposure to physical movement in chronic back pain patients: no evidence for generalization across different movements. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[13]  J. Vlaeyen,et al.  Is pain-related fear a predictor of somatosensory hypervigilance in chronic low back pain patients? , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[14]  E. Fox,et al.  Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  A. Mathews,et al.  Anxiety and Attention to Threatening Pictures , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  R. Dowman Attentional set effects on spinal and supraspinal responses to pain. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[17]  C. Spence,et al.  The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Cognitive-processing bias in chronic pain: a review and integration. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  G. Crombez,et al.  The emotional stroop task and chronic pain: what is threatening for chronic pain sufferers? , 2000, European journal of pain.

[20]  L. Plaghki,et al.  Impaired selective attention in chronic pain patients , 1999, European journal of pain.

[21]  G. Crombez,et al.  Attention to chronic pain is dependent upon pain-related fear. , 1999, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[22]  G. Crombez,et al.  Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[24]  M. Johnson,et al.  The effects of imagery and sensory detection distractors on different measures of pain: how does distraction work? , 1998, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[25]  G. Crombez,et al.  Attentional disruption is enhanced by the threat of pain. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  When somatic information threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference , 1998, Pain.

[27]  G. Crombez,et al.  Attention and somatic awareness in chronic pain , 1997, PAIN.

[28]  L. McCracken “Attention” to pain in persons with chronic pain: A behavioral approach , 1997 .

[29]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. , 1995 .

[30]  C. Eccleston,et al.  Chronic pain and attention: a cognitive approach. , 1994, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[31]  G. Crombez,et al.  Sensory and temporal information about impending pain: the influence of predictability on pain. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[32]  M. Eysenck Anxiety: The Cognitive Perspective , 1992 .

[33]  P. Roland Cortical representation of pain , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain , 1989, Pain.

[35]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[36]  H. Vertommen,et al.  The McGill Pain Questionnaire constructed for the Dutch language (MPQ-DV). Preliminary data concerning reliability and validity , 1987, Pain.

[37]  Albrecht W. Inhoff,et al.  Isolating attentional systems: A cognitive-anatomical analysis , 1987, Psychobiology.

[38]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[39]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys , 1985 .

[40]  K. Mccaul,et al.  Distraction and coping with pain. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[41]  M. Dawson,et al.  Allocation of cognitive processing capacity during human autonomic classical conditioning. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[43]  R. Davidson,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV , 1976 .