Visuo-Spatial Processes as a Domain-General Factor Impacting Numerical Development in Atypical Populations

In the past few years, the role of both domain-specific and domain-general factors on numerical development and mathematics achievement has been debated. In this paper, we focus on the role of visuo-spatial processes. We will more particularly review the numerical abilities of populations presenting atypical visuo-spatial processes: individuals with blindness, hemineglect, children presenting low visuo-spatial abilities, non-verbal learning disorder or Williams syndrome. We will show that math abilities of each population are relatively unique and are not necessarily associated with generalized math impairment. We will show that a better understanding of the strengths and weaknesses of each population gives further insights into our conceptual understanding of the development of numerical cognition. We will finally demonstrate how the comparison across disorders can impact on practical rehabilitation and educational strategies.

[1]  Tracy Packiam Alloway,et al.  Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. , 2007, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Embodied numbers: The role of vision in the development of number–space interactions , 2013, Cortex.

[3]  John C. Marshall,et al.  When Right Goes Left: An Investigation of Line Bisection in a Case of Visual Neglect , 1989, Cortex.

[4]  T. Simon A new account of the neurocognitive foundations of impairments in space, time and number processing in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. , 2008, Developmental disabilities research reviews.

[5]  Julie Castronovo,et al.  Superior numerical abilities following early visual deprivation , 2013, Cortex.

[6]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Typical and Atypical Development of Visual Estimation Abilities , 2007, Cortex.

[7]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Learning of Arbitrary Association between Visual and Auditory Novel Stimuli in Adults: The “Bond Effect” of Haptic Exploration , 2009, PloS one.

[8]  Julie Sarama,et al.  Early Childhood Mathematics Intervention , 2011, Science.

[9]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[10]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[11]  Paola Guariglia,et al.  Spatial Orienting Biases in the Decimal Numeral System , 2009, Current Biology.

[12]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Small and large number processing in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. , 2008, Developmental science.

[13]  C. Semenza,et al.  The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[14]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[15]  C. Semenza,et al.  Numbers in the Blind's “Eye” , 2009, PloS one.

[16]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Laurence Rousselle,et al.  Magnitude Representations in Williams Syndrome: Differential Acuity in Time, Space and Number Processing , 2013, PloS one.

[18]  D. Boisson,et al.  Does Action Make the Link Between Number and Space Representation? , 2004, Psychological science.

[19]  T. Vecchi,et al.  Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[20]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Genetic Syndromes as Model Pathways to Mathematical Learning Difficulties , 2016 .

[21]  Kevin J. Riggs,et al.  Subitizing in congenitally blind adults , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  W. Fias,et al.  Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities: Orientation of the mental number line , 2005 .

[23]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Arithmetic Difficulties in Children With Visuospatial Learning Disability (VLD) , 2003, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[24]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[26]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  An analysis of the criteria used to diagnose children with Nonverbal Learning Disability (NLD) , 2014, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[27]  Masami Ishihara,et al.  Neglect "around the clock": Dissociating number and spatial neglect in right brain damage , 2011 .

[28]  M. Neppi-Modona,et al.  Functional independence between numerical and visual space: Evidence from right brain-damaged patients , 2012, Cortex.

[29]  Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al.  Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task , 2009, Cognition.

[30]  L. Merabet,et al.  The effect of hand movements on numerical bisection judgments in early blind and sighted individuals , 2015, Cortex.

[31]  P. G. Vos,et al.  A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  P. Pantano,et al.  Unilateral neglect restricted to visual imagery , 1993, Nature.

[33]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Vision guides the adjustment of auditory localization in young barn owls. , 1985, Science.

[34]  T. Alloway,et al.  Working Memory and Learning in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder and Specific Language Impairment , 2008, Journal of learning disabilities.

[35]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.

[36]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Mathematical skills in Williams syndrome: insight into the importance of underlying representations. , 2009, Developmental disabilities research reviews.

[37]  D. Cabrol,et al.  Subtypes of Developmental Coordination Disorder: Research on Their Nature and Etiology , 2011, Developmental neuropsychology.

[38]  T. Simon,et al.  Reconceptualizing the Origins of Number Knowledge: A "Non-Numerical" Account , 1997 .

[39]  K. Priftis,et al.  Optokinetic Stimulation Modulates Neglect for the Number Space: Evidence from Mental Number Interval Bisection , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[41]  Wim Fias,et al.  Spatial representation of numbers. , 2004 .

[42]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Spatial bias in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical comparison in neglect , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Wim Fias,et al.  Non-spatial neglect for the mental number line , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Lisa Feigenson,et al.  The equality of quantity , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  J. Hoffman,et al.  Small subitizing range in people with Williams syndrome , 2011, Visual cognition.

[46]  Ulf Träff,et al.  Processing of space, time, and number contributes to mathematical abilities above and beyond domain-general cognitive abilities. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  Michael Andres,et al.  Dissociation of numerosity and duration processing in the left intraparietal sulcus: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2008, Cortex.

[48]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[49]  L. Pelosi,et al.  On the different roles of the cerebral hemispheres in mental imagery: The “o'Clock Test” in two clinical cases , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[50]  M. McCloskey Quantitative literacy and developmental dyscalculias. , 2007 .

[51]  Sara Cordes,et al.  The development of area discrimination and its implications for number representation in infancy. , 2006, Developmental science.

[52]  Valérie Dormal,et al.  Numerosity-length interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[53]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.

[54]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[55]  Olivier Collignon,et al.  Embodied Space in Early Blind Individuals , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[56]  Michael Andres,et al.  Causal role of spatial attention in arithmetic problem solving: Evidence from left unilateral neglect , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Intuitive geometry and visuospatial working memory in children showing symptoms of nonverbal learning disabilities , 2013, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[58]  É. Gentaz,et al.  Évaluation d'entraînements multisensoriels de préparation à la reconnaissance de figures géométriques planes chez les enfants de cinq ans : étude de la contribution du système haptique manuel , 2008 .

[59]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Duration and numerical estimation in right brain-damaged patients with and without neglect: Lack of support for a mental time line. , 2016, British journal of psychology.

[60]  P. Maruff,et al.  Covert orienting of visuospatial attention in children with developmental coordination disorder , 1997, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[61]  Virginie Crollen,et al.  Spatial and numerical processing in children with non-verbal learning disabilities. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.

[62]  G. Vallar,et al.  Listening to numbers affects visual and haptic bisection in healthy individuals and neglect patients , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Valérie Dormal,et al.  A common right fronto‐parietal network for numerosity and duration processing: An fMRI study , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[64]  V. Walsh,et al.  The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  Virginie Crollen,et al.  Spatial and numerical processing in children with high and low visuospatial abilities. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[66]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .

[67]  V. Dormal,et al.  Common and Specific Contributions of the Intraparietal Sulci to Numerosity and Length Processing , 2009, NeuroImage.

[68]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Six-month-old infants use analog magnitudes to represent duration. , 2006, Developmental science.

[69]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: An ERP study , 2008, Cortex.

[70]  E. Gentaz,et al.  Haptics in teaching handwriting: the role of perceptual and visuo-motor skills. , 2011, Human movement science.

[71]  M. Wallace,et al.  Early experience determines how the senses will interact. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  Impairments of the mental number line for two-digit numbers in neglect , 2008, Cortex.

[73]  Daniela Lucangeli,et al.  Spatial Working Memory and Arithmetic Deficits in Children With Nonverbal Learning Difficulties , 2010, Journal of learning disabilities.

[74]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[75]  Mauro Pesenti,et al.  Attentional Bias Induced by Solving Simple and Complex Addition and Subtraction Problems , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[76]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  Different Effects of Numerical Magnitude on Visual and Proprioceptive Reference Frames , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[77]  Fabrizio Doricchi,et al.  Small numbers in the right brain: Evidence from patients without and with spatial neglect , 2013, Cortex.

[78]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[79]  V. Michel,et al.  Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.

[80]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Prism adaptation improves representational neglect , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Semantic numerical representation in blind subjects: The role of vision in the spatial format of the mental number line , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[82]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: Symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.

[83]  Les Staves About number sense , 2018, Very Special Maths.

[84]  Xavier Seron,et al.  The role of vision in the development of finger-number interactions: Finger-counting and finger-montring in blind children. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[85]  F. Lepore,et al.  Visual experience influences the interactions between fingers and numbers , 2014, Cognition.

[86]  U. Cress,et al.  How number-space relationships are assessed before formal schooling: A taxonomy proposal , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[87]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[88]  G. Orban,et al.  Parietal Representation of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Magnitude , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[89]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[90]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Are numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[91]  Franco Lepore,et al.  The ventriloquist in periphery: impact of eccentricity-related reliability on audio-visual localization. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[92]  F. Restle Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .

[93]  M. Dennis,et al.  Mathematical learning disabilities in special populations: phenotypic variation and cross-disorder comparisons. , 2009, Developmental disabilities research reviews.

[94]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[95]  Valéria Csépe,et al.  The parietal distance effect appears in both the congenitally blind and matched sighted controls in an acoustic number comparison task , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[96]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[97]  T. Hung,et al.  The neurophysiological performance of visuospatial working memory in children with developmental coordination disorder , 2012, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[98]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Numbers and time doubly dissociate , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[99]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Dissociations and interactions between time, numerosity and space processing , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[100]  F. Lepore,et al.  Early but not late blindness leads to enhanced arithmetic and working memory abilities , 2016, Cortex.

[101]  R. Cherng,et al.  Mechanisms of deficit of visuospatial attention shift in children with developmental coordination disorder: A neurophysiological measure of the endogenous Posner paradigm , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[102]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Range and regression effects in magnitude scaling , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[103]  Ann Dowker,et al.  Interventions for improving numerical abilities: Present and future , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[104]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[105]  Hans-Christoph Nuerk,et al.  Are Spatial-Numerical Associations a Cornerstone for Arithmetic Learning? The Lack of Genuine Correlations Suggests No , 2015 .

[106]  J. Wearden,et al.  Perception of the Duration of Auditory and Visual Stimuli in Children and Adults , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[107]  Fabrizio Doricchi,et al.  No inherent left and right side in human 'mental number line': evidence from right brain damage. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[108]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[109]  M. Racsmány,et al.  Numerical abilities in Williams syndrome: Dissociating the analogue magnitude system and verbal retrieval , 2009, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[110]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[111]  A. Reiss,et al.  Insights into brain development from neurogenetic syndromes: evidence from fragile X syndrome, Williams syndrome, Turner syndrome and velocardiofacial syndrome , 2009, Neuroscience.

[112]  E. Spelke,et al.  Representations of space, time, and number in neonates , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[113]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[114]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[115]  Paul B. Buckley,et al.  Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[116]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  Space and time in the sighted and blind , 2015, Cognition.

[117]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: Evidence from neglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[118]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Visual instruction of the neural map of auditory space in the developing optic tectum. , 1991, Science.

[119]  Paola Guariglia,et al.  Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[120]  Michael Andres,et al.  Role of distinct parietal areas in arithmetic: An fMRI-guided TMS study , 2011, NeuroImage.

[121]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Regression effect in psychophysical judgment , 1966 .

[122]  B. Landau,et al.  Mathematical skill in individuals with Williams syndrome: Evidence from a standardized mathematics battery , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[123]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[124]  L. Baker Nonverbal learning disabilities: the syndrome and the model. , 1992, Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology.