Absolute pitch in infant auditory learning: evidence for developmental reorganization.

To what extent do infants represent the absolute pitches of complex auditory stimuli? Two experiments with 8-month-old infants examined the use of absolute and relative pitch cues in a tone-sequence statistical learning task. The results suggest that, given unsegmented stimuli that do not conform to the rules of musical composition, infants are more likely to track patterns of absolute pitches than of relative pitches. A 3rd experiment tested adults with or without musical training on the same statistical learning tasks used in the infant experiments. Unlike the infants, adult listeners relied primarily on relative pitch cues. These results suggest a shift from an initial focus on absolute pitch to the eventual dominance of relative pitch, which, it is argued, is more useful for both music and speech processing.

[1]  Jenny R Saffran,et al.  Infant memory for musical experiences , 2000, Cognition.

[2]  I. Peretz,et al.  Absolute pitch in autism: A case study , 1999 .

[3]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  The Beginnings of Word Segmentation in English-Learning Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[5]  Norman M. Weinberger,et al.  Music and the auditory system. , 1999 .

[6]  E. M. Burns Intervals, Scales, and Tuning , 1999 .

[7]  Diana Deutsch,et al.  THE PROCESSING OF PITCH COMBINATIONS , 1999 .

[8]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[9]  Linda Pring,et al.  Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability. , 1998 .

[10]  L. Trainor,et al.  The development of evaluative responses to music:: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance , 1998 .

[11]  Absolute Pitch: Practice Makes Perfect, the Earlier the Better , 1997 .

[12]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  The origins of music perception and cognition: A developmental perspective. , 1997 .

[13]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[14]  Jerome Kagan,et al.  Perception of music by infants , 1996, Nature.

[15]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Natural Musical Intervals: Evidence From Infant Listeners , 1996 .

[16]  S. H. Hulse,et al.  Concurrent absolute and relative pitch processing by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). , 1996 .

[17]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[18]  D. Mewhort,et al.  Classification of frequencies into ranges by songbirds and humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  J. D. Smith,et al.  What child is this? What interval was that? Familiar tunes and music perception in novice listeners , 1994, Cognition.

[20]  D. Levitin Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  What Mediates Infants' and Adults' Superior Processing of the Major over the Augmented Triad? , 1993 .

[22]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words , 1993 .

[23]  S. Trehub,et al.  Musical context effects in infants and adults: key distance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[25]  R E Eilers,et al.  A study of perceptual development for musical tuning , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  D. Deutsch Paradoxes of musical pitch. , 1992, Scientific American.

[27]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. , 1992, Science.

[28]  S. Trehub,et al.  A comparison of infants' and adults' sensitivity to western musical structure. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  K. Stevens,et al.  Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.

[30]  D. Deutsch The Tritone Paradox: An Influence of Language on Music Perception , 1991 .

[31]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[32]  Diana Deutsch,et al.  The Tritone Paradox: Correlate with the Listener's Vocal Range for Speech , 1990 .

[33]  A. Halpern,et al.  Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[34]  K. Miyazaki Musical pitch identification by absolute pitch possessors , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.

[36]  Graham F. Welch,et al.  Observations on the Incidence of Absolute Pitch (AP) Ability in the Early Blind , 1988 .

[37]  S. Trehub,et al.  Infants’ perception of musical patterns , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  S E Trehub,et al.  Organizational processes in infants' perception of auditory patterns. , 1987, Child development.

[39]  S. Trehub,et al.  Infants' perception of musical relations in short transposed tone sequences. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[40]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds , 1986 .

[41]  D. Deutsch A Musical Paradox , 1986 .

[42]  S. Trehub,et al.  Infants' perception of melodies: the role of melodic contour. , 1984, Child development.

[43]  J. Werker,et al.  Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .

[44]  P. Kuhl Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy , 1983 .

[45]  J D Miller,et al.  Discrimination of auditory target dimensions in the presence or absence of variation in a second dimension by infants , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  J. Bartlett,et al.  Recognition of transposed melodies: a key-distance effect in developmental perspective. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[48]  D B Pisoni,et al.  Discimination of relative onset time of two-component tones by infants. , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  P. Kuhl Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[50]  D. Deutsch,et al.  The Psychology of Music , 1983 .

[51]  W. D. Ward,et al.  Categorical perception--phenomenon or epiphenomenon: evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[52]  Sandra E. Trehub,et al.  Auditory Processing of Relational Information by Young Infants , 1977 .

[53]  W. Siegel,et al.  Absolute identification of notes and intervals by musicians , 1977 .

[54]  Annabel J. Cohen,et al.  Recognition of Transposed Melodic Sequences , 1976 .

[55]  E. Terhardt Pitch, consonance, and harmony. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[56]  F. Attneave,et al.  Pitch as a medium: a new approach to psychophysical scaling. , 1971, The American journal of psychology.

[57]  W. Dowling,et al.  Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[58]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.

[59]  D. Deutsch Tones and Numbers: Specificity of Interference in Immediate Memory , 1970, Science.

[60]  D. Sergeant,et al.  Experimental Investigation of Absolute Pitch , 1969 .

[61]  B. White Recognition of distorted melodies. , 1960, The American journal of psychology.