Measures of states of consciousness during attentional and cognitive load

Background Developing and testing methods for reliably assessing states of consciousness in humans is important for both basic research and clinical purposes. Several potential measures, partly grounded in theoretical developments, have been proposed, and some of them seem to reliably distinguish between conscious and unconscious brain states. However, the degrees to which these measures may also be affected by changes in brain activity or conditions that can occur within conscious brain states have rarely been tested. In this study we test whether several of these measures are modulated by attentional load and related use of cognitive resources. Methods We recorded EEG from 12 participants while they passively received three types of stimuli: (1) transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) pulses (for measuring perturbational complexity), (2) auditory stimuli (for detection of auditory pattern deviants), or (3) audible clicks from a clock (spontaneous EEG, for measures of signal diversity and functional connectivity). We investigated whether the measures significantly differed between the passive condition and a attentional and cognitively demanding working memory task. Results Our results showed that in the attention-based auditory P3b ERP measure (global auditory pattern deviant) was significantly affected by increased attentional and cognitive load, while the various measures based on spontaneous and perturbed EEG were not affected. Conclusion Measures of conscious state based on complexity, diversity, and effective connectivity, are not affected by attentional and cognitive load, suggesting that these measures can be used to test both for the presence and absence of consciousness.

[1]  G. E. Smith The Human Brain , 1924, Nature.

[2]  P. Welch The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: A method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms , 1967 .

[3]  B. Jennett,et al.  Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. , 1974, Lancet.

[4]  Abraham Lempel,et al.  On the Complexity of Finite Sequences , 1976, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[5]  HighWire Press The journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. , 1981 .

[6]  Schuster,et al.  Easily calculable measure for the complexity of spatiotemporal patterns. , 1987, Physical review. A, General physics.

[7]  B. A. Blansjaar [Korsakoff syndrome]. , 1993, Nederlands tijdschrift voor geneeskunde.

[8]  M. Kopelman The Korsakoff Syndrome , 1995, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[9]  B. Baars IN THE THEATRE OF CONSCIOUSNESS Global Workspace Theory, A Rigorous Scientific Theory of Consciousness. , 1997 .

[10]  S Dehaene,et al.  A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  L. Rowland Diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis , 1998, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[12]  H. Duvernoy The Human Brain , 1999, Springer Vienna.

[13]  C. Rosow,et al.  Bispectral index monitoring. , 2001, Anesthesiology clinics of North America.

[14]  C. Rosow,et al.  Bispectral index monitoring , 2001 .

[15]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[16]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Attention: From Modulation of Sensory Processing to Top-Down Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Jejo D. Koola,et al.  The Maximum-likelihood Strategy for Determining Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Motor Threshold, Using Parameter Estimation by Sequential Testing Is Faster Than Conventional Methods With Similar Precision , 2004, The journal of ECT.

[18]  K. Leslie,et al.  For Personal Use. Only Reproduce with Permission from the Lancet , 2022 .

[19]  Katarzyna J. Blinowska,et al.  A new method of the description of the information flow in the brain structures , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[20]  R. Blair,et al.  An exact statistical method for comparing topographic maps, with any number of subjects and electrodes , 2005, Brain Topography.

[21]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  P. Rossini,et al.  Antero-posterior functional coupling at sleep onset: changes as a function of increased sleep pressure , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[24]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown of Cortical Effective Connectivity During Sleep , 2005, Science.

[25]  Tarmo Lipping,et al.  Comparison of entropy and complexity measures for the assessment of depth of sedation , 2006, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[26]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Brain connectivity , 2007, Scholarpedia.

[27]  F. Hyder,et al.  Baseline brain energy supports the state of consciousness , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  G. Tononi,et al.  Slow waves, synaptic plasticity and information processing: insights from transcranial magnetic stimulation and high‐density EEG experiments , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[32]  T. Kretschmer,et al.  Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology , 2010 .

[33]  M. Shanahan Metastable chimera states in community-structured oscillator networks. , 2009, Chaos.

[34]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain Connectivity , 2011, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[36]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Neural Oscillations Associated with Item and Temporal Order Maintenance in Working Memory , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Jiann-Shing Shieh,et al.  Comparison of EEG Approximate Entropy and Complexity Measures of Depth of Anaesthesia During Inhalational General Anaesthesia , 2011 .

[38]  Laura Astolfi,et al.  Connectome : A MATLAB toolbox for mapping and imaging of brain , 2010 .

[39]  G. Tononi,et al.  Unresponsiveness versus Unconsciousness , 2012 .

[40]  R. E. Greenblatt,et al.  Connectivity measures applied to human brain electrophysiological data , 2012, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[41]  G. Tononi,et al.  Unresponsiveness ≠ Unconsciousness , 2012, Anesthesiology.

[42]  J. R. King,et al.  Single-trial decoding of auditory novelty responses facilitates the detection of residual consciousness , 2013, NeuroImage.

[43]  G. Tononi,et al.  A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior , 2013, Science Translational Medicine.

[44]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Connectivity biomarkers can differentiate patients with different levels of consciousness , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  Larissa Albantakis,et al.  From the Phenomenology to the Mechanisms of Consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0 , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[46]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval , 2014, NeuroImage.

[47]  M. Boly,et al.  Complexity of Multi-Dimensional Spontaneous EEG Decreases during Propofol Induced General Anaesthesia , 2015, PloS one.

[48]  P. J. Schuller,et al.  Response of bispectral index to neuromuscular block in awake volunteers. , 2015, British journal of anaesthesia.

[49]  G. Tononi,et al.  Propofol anesthesia reduces Lempel-Ziv complexity of spontaneous brain activity in rats , 2016, Neuroscience Letters.

[50]  G. Tononi,et al.  Stratification of unresponsive patients by an independently validated index of brain complexity , 2016, Annals of neurology.

[51]  A. Seth,et al.  Global and local complexity of intracranial EEG decreases during NREM sleep , 2017, Neuroscience of consciousness.

[52]  M. Schartner On the relation between complex brain activity and consciousness , 2017 .

[53]  A. Seth,et al.  Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[54]  B. E. Juel,et al.  Distinguishing Anesthetized from Awake State in Patients: A New Approach Using One Second Segments of Raw EEG , 2018, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[55]  A. Fornito,et al.  Sensory contamination in TMS-EEG recordings: Can we isolate TMS-evoked neural activity? , 2019, Brain Stimulation.

[56]  Hartwig R. Siebner,et al.  The non-transcranial TMS-evoked potential is an inherent source of ambiguity in TMS-EEG studies , 2018, NeuroImage.

[57]  Risto J. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Reproducibility in TMS–EEG studies: a call for data sharing, standard procedures and effective experimental control , 2019 .

[58]  B. E. Juel,et al.  Increased signal diversity/complexity of spontaneous EEG, but not evoked EEG responses, in ketamine-induced psychedelic state in humans , 2019, bioRxiv.