Modeling the effects of methylphenidate on interference and evidence accumulation processes using the conflict linear ballistic accumulator

RationaleAlthough methylphenidate and other stimulants have been demonstrated to improve task performance across a variety of domains, a computationally rigorous account of how these drugs alter cognitive processing remains elusive. Recent applications of mathematical models of cognitive processing and electrophysiological methods to this question have suggested that stimulants improve the integrity of evidence accumulation processes for relevant choices, potentially through catecholaminergic modulation of neural signal-to-noise ratios. However, this nascent line of work has thus far been limited to simple perceptual tasks and has largely omitted more complex conflict paradigms that contain experimental manipulations of specific top-down interference resolution processes.Objectives and methodsTo address this gap, this study applied the conflict linear ballistic accumulator (LBA), a newly proposed model designed for conflict tasks, to data from healthy adults who performed the Multi-Source Interference Task (MSIT) after acute methylphenidate or placebo challenge.ResultsModel-based analyses revealed that methylphenidate improved performance by reducing individuals’ response thresholds and by enhancing evidence accumulation processes across all task conditions, either by improving the quality of evidence or by reducing variability in accumulation processes. In contrast, the drug did not reduce bottom-up interference or selectively facilitate top-down interference resolution processes probed by the experimental conflict manipulation.ConclusionsEnhancement of evidence accumulation is a biologically plausible and task-general mechanism of stimulant effects on cognition. Moreover, the assumption that methylphenidate’s effects on behavior are only visible with complex executive tasks may be misguided.

[1]  N. G. Best,et al.  The deviance information criterion: 12 years on , 2014 .

[2]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A diffusion model decomposition of the practice effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[6]  S. Kelly,et al.  Internal and External Influences on the Rate of Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[8]  B. Pennington,et al.  Validity of the Executive Function Theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  S. Pliszka,et al.  Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2007, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[10]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Diffusion models of the flanker task: Discrete versus gradual attentional selection , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. , 2013, Psychological methods.

[12]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of methylphenidate and placebo in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the multi-source interference task. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  C. White,et al.  Acute Stimulant Treatment and Reinforcement Increase the Speed of Information Accumulation in Children with ADHD , 2017, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[15]  M. Steinhauser,et al.  A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. , 2010, Psychological review.

[16]  Sverker Sikström,et al.  Stimulus-dependent dopamine release in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2007, Psychological review.

[17]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Estimating across-trial variability parameters of the Diffusion Decision Model: Expert advice and recommendations , 2018, Journal of Mathematical Psychology.

[18]  Young T. Hong,et al.  Dopamine Release in Dissociable Striatal Subregions Predicts the Different Effects of Oral Methylphenidate on Reversal Learning and Spatial Working Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  P. Snyder,et al.  Cognitive effects of immediate-release methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The overconstraint of response time models: Rethinking the scaling problem , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  W. Pelham,et al.  Reinforcement and Stimulant Medication Ameliorate Deficient Response Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2016, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[24]  J. Borkowski,et al.  Effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin) on information processing in hyperactive children , 1984, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[25]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Méadhbh B. Brosnan,et al.  Catecholamine Modulation of Evidence Accumulation during Perceptual Decision Formation: A Randomized Trial , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  R. Ivry,et al.  Focal striatum lesions impair cautiousness in humans , 2016, Cortex.

[28]  Bruno Kopp,et al.  Data quality over data quantity in computational cognitive neuroscience , 2018, NeuroImage.

[29]  Christopher R. Myers,et al.  Universally Sloppy Parameter Sensitivities in Systems Biology Models , 2007, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[30]  Sumio Watanabe,et al.  Asymptotic Equivalence of Bayes Cross Validation and Widely Applicable Information Criterion in Singular Learning Theory , 2010, J. Mach. Learn. Res..

[31]  G. Bush,et al.  The Multi-Source Interference Task: an fMRI task that reliably activates the cingulo-frontal-parietal cognitive/attention network , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[32]  Xiao-Li Meng,et al.  POSTERIOR PREDICTIVE ASSESSMENT OF MODEL FITNESS VIA REALIZED DISCREPANCIES , 1996 .

[33]  I. Locatelli,et al.  Comparative efficacy and acceptability of atomoxetine, lisdexamfetamine, bupropion and methylphenidate in treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis with focus on bupropion. , 2015, Journal of affective disorders.

[34]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[35]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  Improving Working Memory in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: The Separate and Combined Effects of Incentives and Stimulant Medication , 2012, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[37]  J. Currie,et al.  Effects of Methylphenidate on Cognitive Functions in Children and Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Evidence from a Systematic Review and a Meta-Analysis , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[38]  Mads L Pedersen,et al.  Modelling ADHD: A review of ADHD theories through their predictions for computational models of decision-making and reinforcement learning , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  M. Solanto Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms of stimulant drug action in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review and integration , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. , 2015, Psychological review.

[41]  V. Russell,et al.  A behavioral neuroenergetics theory of ADHD , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[42]  W. Pelham,et al.  How do stimulant treatments for ADHD work? Evidence for mediation by improved cognition , 2018, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[43]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  H. Akaike Maximum likelihood identification of Gaussian autoregressive moving average models , 1973 .

[45]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[46]  Stephan Bender,et al.  Annual research review: Reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders: measurement and mechanisms of a proposed trans-diagnostic phenotype. , 2014, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[47]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[48]  A. Hedge,et al.  The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. , 1975, Acta psychologica.

[49]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  An introduction to good practices in cognitive modeling , 2015 .

[50]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Dynamic models of choice , 2018, Behavior Research Methods.

[51]  Jonathon Love,et al.  Decision processes and the slowing of simple choices in schizophrenia. , 2015, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[52]  S. Zysset,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of brain systems subserving decision making under uncertainty: A study with fMRI and methylphenidate challenge , 2009, Synapse.

[53]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Testing Formal Predictions of Neuroscientific Theories of ADHD With a Cognitive Model–Based Approach , 2018, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[54]  D. Rubin,et al.  Inference from Iterative Simulation Using Multiple Sequences , 1992 .

[55]  Aki Vehtari,et al.  Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC , 2015, Statistics and Computing.

[56]  G. Logan,et al.  Testing the validity of conflict drift-diffusion models for use in estimating cognitive processes: A parameter-recovery study , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.