Experiential Learning, Spatial Visualization and Metacognition: An Exercise with the “Blank Page” Technique for Learning Anatomy

Abstract Purpose Undergraduate students in the allied health science disciplines are normally required to complete a human anatomy course in their first year. Such courses, while popular, present challenges in that the content requires students to engage different approaches to learning. Recent literature reflects an increased interest in active, experiential learning activities to improve the learning of anatomy and physiology, but activities that focus on the development of metacognitive skills and visual-spatial thinking have been lacking. To address these inadequacies we developed a plasticine modeling and drawing activity in a tutorial room devoid of resources and visual cues referred to as the “blank page” room. The purpose of this manuscript is to communicate the merits of this intervention as an instructional technique used to facilitate the learning of anatomy for undergraduates of allied health disciplines. Method During anatomy laboratory sessions we randomly allocated student groups to the activity of plasticine modeling and drawing (blank page technique) or the completion of written review questions. We compared the grades achieved by students who had been exposed to the blank page intervention with the grades achieved by students who were given standard review questions. We also collected qualitative feedback in the form of questionnaires that required participating students to rate the learning efficacy of the activities. Results Students performed slightly better on assessment quizzes after the blank page activity compared with the review questions. Student feedback indicated that the blank page activity had greater learning value and promoted stronger engagement in the learning tasks. Discussion The blank page activity has merit in student engagement and facilitation of the learning of anatomy by broadening the scope of instruction to encompass multimodal learning preferences, metacognition and visual spatial thinking.

[1]  Gregory Schraw A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring , 2009 .

[2]  D. Kolb Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development , 1983 .

[3]  Neil D. Fleming,et al.  Not Another Inventory, Rather a Catalyst for Reflection , 1992 .

[4]  Pamela White,et al.  The impact of color and role on retention of knowledge: A body‐painting study within undergraduate medicine , 2011, Anatomical sciences education.

[5]  Carina Endres Howell,et al.  Sectioning Clay Models Makes Anatomy & Development Tangible , 2010 .

[6]  J. Marone,et al.  Clay modeling versus written modules as effective interventions in understanding human anatomy , 2013, Anatomical sciences education.

[7]  G. Norman,et al.  How medical students learn spatial anatomy , 2001, The Lancet.

[8]  Improving first-year laboratory classes in bioscience – students’ views , 2009 .

[9]  Amy Skinder-Meredith Innovative activities for teaching anatomy of speech production , 2010, Anatomical sciences education.

[10]  T. Lord Enhancing learning in the life sciences through spatial perception , 1990 .

[11]  R. Felder,et al.  Understanding Student Differences , 2005 .

[12]  Helen L. Naug,et al.  Promoting metacognition in first year anatomy laboratories using plasticine modeling and drawing activities: A pilot study of the “Blank Page” technique , 2011, Anatomical sciences education.

[13]  Leon Piterman,et al.  Riding the wave: current and emerging trends in graduates from Australian university medical schools , 2007, The Medical journal of Australia.

[14]  Anastasia S. Morrone,et al.  Theoretical Eclecticism in the College Classroom , 2005 .

[15]  J. Kooloos,et al.  Learning anatomy enhances spatial ability , 2013, Anatomical sciences education.

[16]  J. Mathewson Visual-spatial thinking: An aspect of science overlooked by educators , 1999 .

[17]  C. Fosnot Constructivism: a psychological theory of learning , 1996 .

[18]  Ji-Young Kim,et al.  Learning of cross‐sectional anatomy using clay models , 2009, Anatomical sciences education.

[19]  Lora T. Likova Drawing enhances cross-modal memory plasticity in the human brain: a case study in a totally blind adult , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  Peter J. Ward,et al.  The influence of study methods and knowledge processing on academic success and long‐term recall of anatomy learning by first‐year veterinary students , 2008, Anatomical sciences education.

[21]  Kent J. Crippen,et al.  Promoting Self-Regulation in Science Education: Metacognition as Part of a Broader Perspective on Learning , 2006 .

[22]  D. R. Montello,et al.  The Role of Spatial Cognition in Medicine: Applications for Selecting and Training Professionals. , 2007 .

[23]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Individual differences in use of external visualisations to perform an internal visualisation task. , 2007 .

[24]  H. Motoike,et al.  Clay modeling as a method to learn human muscles: A community college study , 2009, Anatomical sciences education.

[25]  L. Montplaisir,et al.  Students' illustrations of the human nervous system as a formative assessment tool , 2010, Anatomical sciences education.

[26]  S. DiCarlo,et al.  First-year medical students prefer multiple learning styles. , 2006, Advances in physiology education.

[27]  Gabrielle M Finn,et al.  A qualitative study of student responses to body painting , 2009, Anatomical sciences education.

[28]  M. Niemivirta,et al.  Interrelations among university students’ approaches to learning, regulation of learning, and cognitive and attributional strategies: a person oriented approach , 2011 .