Syntactic Bootstrapping in theAcquisition of Attitude Verbs: Think, Want and Hope

Attitude verbs, like think, want and hope, seem to be acquired later than other verbs. Moreover, some attitude verbs (want) seem to be acquired later than others (think). Why should this be? Perhaps observability is a bigger problem than in other cases of word-learning. Psychological events like thinking and wanting do not have direct physical correlates unlike objects like dog or cat, or events like running or eating. Although on the surface, this seems to be a huge problem, it may not be as difficult as it initially appears to be. Advances in cognitive development have revealed that from a very young age, all humans share common conceptual and perceptual capacities, which have been argued to weaken the force of Quine’s famous induction problem. From just a few months old, infants have rich concepts of continuity of objects, gravity, event representation and more (Behl-Chadha 1996, Carey 2009, Eimas & Miller 1990, Mandler 1988, Quinn & Eimas 1986, Soja, Carey & Spelke 1991, Spleke & Kestenbaum 1986, Spelke, Kestenbaun, Simons & Wein 1995, Xu & Carey 1996, and others), and these concepts shape their perception and attention in a way that may restrict candidate word meanings. In addition, infants have been shown to track the minds of others with astonishing ease. From the first months of life, infants give privileged status to human agents, and are sensitive to conversation partners’ goals and perspectives. They attribute goal-directedness to human agents from as young as 5 months of age (Woodward 1996). This sensitivity to other people’s minds also aids children in language acquisition. Children can track eye-gaze and use it to learn new words by 16 months old (Baldwin 1991, Bloom 2000, Moore & Corkum 1994, Smith, Jones & Landau 1996, Plunkett 1997). By age 2, children are adult-like in their interpretation of indexical pronouns, which shift reference based on conversational roles (Moyer, Harrigan, Hacquard & Lidz 2014). Given the salience of psychological states in infant reasoning, linking psychological concepts with words may be no more difficult than linking object and event concepts with words, despite the lack of physical evidence in the world. If, due to this richness of the learners’ representations of other minds, this linking is indeed straightforward, then there is no conceptual barrier to learning attitude verbs. Indeed, some attitude verbs do seem to be acquired easily. Children start producing want to express desires as young as 18 months (Bartsch & Wellman 1995). They further seem to be very good at understanding sentences with want by around 3 years of age (Bartsch & Wellman 1995; Hadwin & Perner 1991; Harrigan, Hacquard & Lidz, submitted; Repacholi & Gopnik 1997; Stein & Levine 1989; Wellman & Banerjee 1991; Wellman & Bartsch 1988; Wellman & Wooley 1990, Yuill 1984; and others). This suggests that the concept of desire is readily available to very young children, that they are proficient from a very young age at tracking others’ minds, and that their sophisticated cognitive capacities allow them to link the word want to the concept of desire with relative ease. For the verb think, however, we see a different trajectory. Many studies have found that children have difficulty understanding sentences with think well into their fourth year, in particular when the sentences refer to someone’s false belief (de Villiers 1995; 2005; 2007; de Villiers & de Villiers 2000; de Villiers & Pyers 2002; Johnson & Maratsos 1977; Lewis 2013; Perner et al. 2003; Wellman, Cross & Watson

[1]  Mandy Simons,et al.  Observations on embedding verbs, evidentiality, and presupposition , 2007 .

[2]  de Villiers,et al.  Can Language Acquisition Give Children a Point of View , 2005 .

[3]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[4]  M. Maratsos,et al.  Early comprehension of mental verbs: Think and know , 1977 .

[5]  H. Wellman,et al.  Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires , 1991 .

[6]  Tatjana Scheffler Semantic operators in different dimensions , 2008 .

[7]  Irene Heim,et al.  Presupposition Projection and the Semantics of Attitude Verbs , 1992, J. Semant..

[8]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[9]  B. Landau Language and experience , 1985 .

[10]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  V. Hacquard,et al.  Three-Year-Olds’ Understanding of Know and Think , 2015 .

[12]  Jill de Villiers,et al.  The Role of Language in Theory of Mind Development. , 2014 .

[13]  C. Heyes False belief in infancy: a fresh look. , 2014, Developmental science.

[14]  S. Pinker Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .

[15]  Hubert Truckenbrodt,et al.  On the semantic motivation of syntactic verb movement to C in German , 2006 .

[16]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping , 1996, Cognition.

[17]  A. Leslie,et al.  Attending to and learning about mental states , 2000 .

[18]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Syntactic Supports for Word Learning , 2008 .

[19]  Josef Perner,et al.  Implicit Understanding of Belief , 1994 .

[20]  John H. Flavell,et al.  The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. , 1988 .

[21]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. , 2003, Developmental science.

[22]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[23]  J. Hooper On Assertive Predicates , 1975 .

[24]  J. Hintikka Semantics for Propositional Attitudes , 1969 .

[25]  C. Fisher,et al.  Two-year-olds use distributional cues to interpret transitivity-alternating verbs , 2009, Language and cognitive processes.

[26]  Willard Van Orman Quine,et al.  Word and Object , 1960 .

[27]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[28]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.

[29]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[30]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[31]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Theories of early language acquisition , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  J. Villiers The interface of language and Theory of Mind , 2007 .

[33]  E. Villalta Mood and gradability: an investigation of the subjunctive mood in Spanish , 2008 .

[34]  M. Tomasello,et al.  “This way!”, “No! That way!”—3-year olds know that two people can have mutually incompatible desires , 2007 .

[35]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[36]  V. Hacquard,et al.  Epistemics and attitudes , 2013 .

[37]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[38]  H. Wellman,et al.  From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology , 1990, Cognition.

[39]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception, ontology, and word meaning , 1992, Cognition.

[40]  Kristen Nicole Asplin Can complement frames help children learn the meaning of abstract verbs , 2002 .

[41]  A. Giannakidou The Landscape of Polarity Items , 1997 .

[42]  Elisabeth Villalta,et al.  Spanish Subjunctive Clauses Require Ordered Alternatives , 2000 .

[43]  H. Wellman,et al.  Children Talk About the Mind , 1995 .

[44]  André Meinunger Interface restrictions on verb second , 2006 .

[45]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Human simulations of vocabulary learning , 1999, Cognition.

[46]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  The semantics and pragmatics of belief reports in preschoolers , 2012 .

[47]  D. Bolinger,et al.  语言要略 = Aspects of Language , 1968 .

[48]  D. Farkas Intensional descriptions and the romance subjunctive mood , 1985 .

[49]  Rory T. Devine,et al.  Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Child development.

[50]  E. Spelke Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge , 1998 .

[51]  Shevaun Lewis,et al.  Pragmatic enrichment in language processing and development , 2013 .

[52]  Daniel Vanderveken,et al.  Foundations of Illocutionary Logic , 1985 .

[53]  Josef Perner,et al.  Want that is understood well before say that, think that, and false belief: a test of de Villiers's linguistic determinism on German-speaking children. , 2003, Child development.

[54]  Sylvia Yuan,et al.  “Really? She Blicked the Baby?” , 2009, Psychological science.

[55]  Josef Perner,et al.  Pleased and surprised: Children's cognitive theory of emotion , 1991 .

[56]  H. Wellman,et al.  The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state , 1983, Cognition.

[57]  L. Gleitman,et al.  When we think about thinking: The acquisition of belief verbs , 2007, Cognition.

[58]  E. Kako,et al.  First contact in verb acquisition: defining a role for syntax. , 1993, Child development.

[59]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Is agency skin-deep? Surface attributes influence infants' sensitivity to goal-directed action , 2004 .

[60]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Young children's reasoning about beliefs , 1988, Cognition.

[61]  Ann Senghas,et al.  Language Promotes False-Belief Understanding , 2009, Psychological science.

[62]  C. Moore,et al.  Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .

[63]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatiotemporal continuity, smoothness of motion and object identity in infancy , 1995 .

[64]  Jennie E. Pyers,et al.  Complements to cognition: a longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding , 2002 .

[65]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[66]  E. Spelke,et al.  Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.

[67]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar , 2003, Cognition.

[68]  C. Moore,et al.  Conflicting desire and the child's theory of mind , 1995 .

[69]  Letitia R. Naigles Manipulating the input: Studies in mental verb acquisition. , 2000 .

[70]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  The role of incremental parsing in syntactically conditioned word learning , 2017, Cognitive Psychology.

[71]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[72]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[73]  J. Perner,et al.  Infants' Insight into the Mind: How Deep? , 2005, Science.

[74]  Nancy L. Stein,et al.  The Causal Organisation of Emotional Knowledge: A Developmental Study , 1989 .