The roles of altitude and fear in the perception of height.

Previous research on perceiving spatial layout has found that people often exhibit normative biases in their perception of the environment. For instance, slant is typically overestimated and distance is usually underestimated. Surprisingly, however, the perception of height has rarely been studied. The present experiments examined the perception of height when viewed from the top (e.g., looking down) or from the bottom (e.g., looking up). Multiple measures were adapted from previous studies of horizontal extents to assess the perception of height. Across all of the measures, a large, consistent bias was found: Vertical distances were greatly overestimated, especially from the top. Secondary findings suggest that the overestimation of distance and size that occurs when looking down from a high place correlates with reports of trait- and state-level fear of heights, suggesting that height overestimation may be due, in part, to fear.

[1]  Lafayette Houghton Bunnell Discovery Of The Yosemite And The Indian War Of 1851 Which Led To That Event , 1880 .

[2]  Alfred H. Holway,et al.  Determinants of Apparent Visual Size with Distance Variant , 1941 .

[3]  A. S. Gilinsky Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.

[4]  W. H. Ittelson,et al.  The size-distance invariance hypothesis. , 1953, Psychological review.

[5]  A. S. Gilinsky,et al.  The effect of attitude upon the perception of size. , 1955, The American journal of psychology.

[6]  W. Epstein,et al.  The current status of the size-distance hypotheses. , 1961, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  W. Gogel,et al.  EQUIDISTANCE TENDENCY AND ITS CONSEQUENCES. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  L O Harvey,et al.  Size matching as a function of instructions in a naturalistic environment. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  L O Harvey,et al.  Effect of instructions, environment, and type of test object on matched size. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  W. Epstein,et al.  The Process of ‘Taking-into-Account’ in Visual Perception , 1973, Perception.

[11]  W. Epstein Stability and constancy in visual perception : mechanisms and processes , 1977 .

[12]  D. C. Cohen,et al.  Comparison of self-report and overt-behavioral procedures for assessing acrophobia. , 1977 .

[13]  P. Lang,et al.  Psychophysiology of fear imagery: differences between focal phobia and social performance anxiety. , 1978, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[14]  O. Sabouraud [Space perception]. , 1978, Revue d'oto-neuro-ophtalmologie.

[15]  Walter C. Gogel,et al.  A comparison of oculomotor and motion parallax cues of egocentric distance , 1979, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Thomson How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  A. Patla,et al.  Locomotor Estimation of Distance after Visual Scanning by Children and Adults , 1985, Perception.

[19]  Kenneth R. Boff,et al.  Sensory processes and perception , 1986 .

[20]  Andrew Rutherford Handbook of perception and human performance. Vol 1: Sensory processes and perception. Vol 2: Cognitive processes and performance. , 1987 .

[21]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  H. Hekmat Origins and development of human fear reactions , 1987 .

[23]  M A Goodale,et al.  The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[25]  S. Rachman,et al.  Fearful distortions. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Clarke,et al.  Danger expectancies and insight in acrophobia. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[28]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .

[29]  Rich Gossweiler,et al.  Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  J S Tittle,et al.  The visual perception of three-dimensional length. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  S S Fukusima,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Judith D. Singer,et al.  Using SAS PROC MIXED to Fit Multilevel Models, Hierarchical Models, and Individual Growth Models , 1998 .

[35]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.

[36]  J. Loomis,et al.  Reproduction of Object Shape is More Accurate without the Continued Availability of Visual Information , 1998, Perception.

[37]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Seeing Big Things: Overestimation of Heights is Greater for Real Objects Than for Objects in Pictures , 1999, Perception.

[38]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Mapping the Zone of Eye-Height Utility for Seated and Standing Observers , 2000, Perception.

[39]  Raôul R. D. Oudejans,et al.  Anxiety–performance relationships in climbing: a process-oriented approach , 2003 .

[40]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.

[41]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perceiving Distance: A Role of Effort and Intent , 2004, Perception.

[42]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Distances appear different on hills , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  J. Riskind,et al.  The loomingness of danger: Does it discriminate focal phobia and general anxiety from depression? , 1992, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[44]  S. Cotler,et al.  Problem-solving skills training as a supplement to behavioral parent training , 1992, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[45]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Reply to Hutchison and Loomis , 2006, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[46]  J. Andre,et al.  Using verbal and blind-walking distance estimates to investigate the two visual systems hypothesis , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[47]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  The Role of Anxiety in Perceiving and Realizing Affordances , 2006 .

[48]  Lawrence K Cormack,et al.  Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Skating down a Steeper Slope: Fear Influences the Perception of Geographical Slant , 2008, Perception.

[50]  T. Martin,et al.  Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy , 2008 .

[51]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  A new mode of fear expression: perceptual bias in height fear. , 2008, Emotion.

[52]  P. Beek,et al.  Affective stimulus properties influence size perception and the Ebbinghaus illusion , 2007, Psychological research.

[53]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Imagery and fear influence height perception. , 2009, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[54]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Don't look down: emotional arousal elevates height perception. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[55]  Angle of elevation influences distance perception to targets on the ceiling , 2010 .